INTERGENERATIONAL PROGRAMMING: ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN AND PERCEPTIONS OF OLDER ADULT PARTICIPANTS by Shannon Ableson-Toronitz B.Sc., University of Victoria, 1993 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF EDUCATION in COUNSELING © ShannonAbleson-Toronitz THE UNIVERSITY OF NORTHERN BRITISH COLUMBIA May, 1997 All rights reserved. This work may not be reproduced in whole or in part, by photocopy or other means without the permission of the author. Abstract The purpose of this study was to extend the intergenerational research by examining children's attitudes toward older adults and older adults' perceptions of children following participation in intergenerational programs. The connection between children's attitudes toward the elderly and the characteristics and perceptions of older adults in intergenerational programs appears to be overlooked in the intergenerational literature. The question arises whether intergenerational contact within long-term care facilities can result in childrens' positive attitudes toward the elderly when participants in nursing homes tend to fulfill the negative physical and behavioral stereotypes that perpetuate negative attitudes toward the elderly. Thirty-six, elementary students and twenty elderly institutionalized residents participated bi-weekly in two programs: (a) a general program with residents in a main facility, and (b) a dementia program with residents in a dementia unit. Students completed the modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students for the purpose of deductively examining the relationships between the variables of gender, program affiliation, and number of years of participation. Statistical analyses identified that: (a) there was no significant difference in attitude between students in the general and dementia programs, (b) there was no significant difference in attitude between male and female students, and (c) students participating for two years demonstrated a significantly more positive attitude than did those participating for one year. Students in both the general and dementia programs provided more positive than negative responses when they were asked what they liked and did not like about the programs. Residents in the general program were interviewed using the modified Older Adult Interview for the purpose of deductively exploring the perceptions of the residents tow~rd the children and the program. Residents in the general program provided more positive responses than either negative or neutral responses in reference to the program and the children. In the general program, both the residents and the students identified many aspects of the program that they liked and only a few aspects they disliked. lll CONTENTS Abstract ................................................................................................. iii Contents ................................................................................................. iv ListofTables ........................................................................................... vi Acknowledgments ..................................................................................... vii Chapter One INTRODUCTION .............................................................. 1 lntergenerational Programming ........................................... 2 Purpose of Research ........................................................ 4 Chapter Two REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ........................................... 6 lntergenerational Contact in Society ...................................... 6 lntergenerational Programming ........................................... 9 Attitudes ...................................................................... 15 Childrens' Attitudes Prior to Intergenerational Programming ......... 15 Childrens' Attitudes and lntergenerational Programming .............. 21 Perceptions of Older Adults and Intergenerational Programming ..... 30 Summary of the Literature Review ....................................... 35 Chapter Three MEfHOD ....................................................................... 38 Subjects ...................................................................... 39 Instruments .................................................................. 42 Research Design ............................................................ 43 Procedures ................................................................... 45 Chapter Four RESULTS ........................................................................ 53 Item Analysis of the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ............................................. 53 Results of the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ........................................................... 55 Results of the Modified Older Adult Interview .......................... 59 Chapter Five DISCUSSION ................................................................... 61 Overview of Study .......................................................... 61 Results ....................................................................... 62 Research Results and the lntergenerational Literature .................. 63 Limitations ................................................................... 65 Areas for Future Research ................................................. 68 Summary ..................................................................... 70 References ...................................................................................... : ....... 71 lV Appendices APPENDIX A- Items and Standard Explanations for the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ............. 78 APPENDIX B- Modified Older Adult Interview ........................... 81 APPENDIX C- Letter of Consent for Parents .............................. 82 APPENDIX D- Letter of Consent for Hillside School, Valleyview Lodge, and School District #57 ............................. 84 APPENDIX E- Statistical Formulas and Formula Definitions in Analysis ................................................................... 85 APPENDIX F- Item Analysis of Responses to the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ........................ 87 APPENDIX G- Proportion of Students Responding Positively and Neutrally on the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ................................... 90 APPENDIX H- Summary of Codeable Responses to the Modified Older Adult Interview ................................... . ....... 93 APPENDIX 1- Distribution of Codeable Responses to the Modified Older Adult Interview ........................................... 94 APPENDIX J- Summary of Codeable Responses to Question Eight of the Modified Older Adult Interview ................. 95 APPENDIX K- Summary of Codeable Responses to Question Twelve of the Modified Older Adult Interview .......................... 96 v TABLES 1. Summary of General and Dementia Intergenerational Participants ...................... 39 2. Summary of Characteristics of General and Dementia Programs ....................... 40 3. Positive Descriptive Responses from General Program Residents to the Question, "Can you describe your buddy for me?" ............................... 52 4. A Summary of the Item Discrimination Results of the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ......................................... 55 5. Distribution of Student Scores on the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire ....................................................................... 56 6. Frequencies for Responses to the Question, "What do you not like about visiting with the senior citizens at the Valleyview Lodge?" ..... . ........................ 57 7. Program Responses to the Question, "What do you like about visiting . cttlzens .. . Lod ge.?" . ................................................ . 58 semor atthe all eyvtew 8. Summary of Codeable Responses from Older Adults in the General Program .............................................................................. 59 v Vl ACKNOWLEOOMENTS Thank you to Bill Knudsen and Betty Rajotte at Valleyview Lodge as well as Diane Rockwell and Jody Maher at Hillside Elementary for their participation in this study. Thank you to Bryan Hartman for his consistently high standards of supervision and my committee for their assistance and support throughout this research. Finally, a special thank you to Serena Ableson for her technical support and Dale Toronitz for his ongoing encouragement . Vll 1 CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCfiON An increasingly aging society coupled with changes in family structure have led to new strategies for individuals from different generations with diverse backgrounds and interests to experience positive and mutually-beneficial interactions (Seefeldt, 1989; Ventura-Merkel, Liederman & Ossofsky, 1989; Cherry, Benest, Gates & White, 1985). Intergenerational programs, or intergenerational remotivational therapy (Allred & Dobson, 1987) brings together children and older adults to share mutual interests and "recreate the once natural connections between old and young" (Seefeldt, 1989, p.186). Intergenerational programming, based on activity theory, bridges the generational gap by incorporating activities that encourage interactions between young and old (Haber & ShortDeGraf, 1990). By bringing old and young together through intergenerational programs, both groups can benefit by having their physical and social needs met (Haber & ShortDeGraf, 1990; Seefeldt, 1989; Newman, Lyons & Onawola, 1985) while helping children develop more positive concepts of aging (Allred & Dobson, 1987; Serock, Seefeldt, Jantz & Galper, 1977). It seems that the overall benefits of intergenerational programming outweigh the negative findings (Haber & Short-DeGraf, 1990; Seefeldt, 1989; Newman et al., 1985). However, there is conflicting evidence as to the usefulness of intergenerational programs in promoting children's attitudes towards the elderly (Fox & Giles, 1993; Haber & ShortDeGraf, 1990; Seefeldt, 1987a) and the value to the elderly participants (Kuehne, 1992). This study will examine the attitudes of elementary school children involved in two longterm care intergenerational programs and the perceptions of the elderly participants regarding their participation in the programs. 2 Intergenerational Programming Defining Intergenerational Programs Theoretical and operational definitions of intergenerational programs across studies have resulted in inconsistencies among research outcomes. Seefeldt ( 1989) describes intergenerational programs as recreating "the once natural connections between the young and old" (p. 186). Kuehne (1992), Hill (1987), and Henkin and Sweeney (1989) define intergenerational programs in a similar manner. Allred and Dobson (1987) describe "intergenerational remotivation therapy [as that which] involves the pairing of children with older adults to discuss topics of mutual interest" (p. 217). More specifically, intergenerational programs "challenge typical stereotypes and attempt to eliminate biases of each generation about the other by facilitating situations for the young and old to interact" (Haber & Short-De Graf, 1990, p. 37). Keller (1990) defines intergenerational programming as, "opportunities to create mutual support between generations and increase familiarity and understanding of human development on the life continuum" (p. 314). Chapman and Neal (1990) and Fox and Giles (1993) identify intergenerational programs as organized activities established to bring together two generations for the purpose of attitude change. Finally, a guidebook for intergenerational programming published by the Government of Ontario (1983) defines intergenerational programming as, "the deliberate organization of non-age specific program opportunities of both a structured and non-structured nature" (p. 13). The diversity in theoretical definitions of intergenerational programs reflects the diverse operational definitions used to empirically define this construct. Intergenerational programs range from education curriculums in the classroom without intergenerational contact to day care facilities within long-term care facilities. Settings for intergenerational contact include long-term care facilities, day care centers for 3 children and older adults, schools, churches, and the outdoors. Participants in intergenerational programs range from pre-school children to university students, and elderly residents in long-term care facilities to independent elderly persons in retirement complexes. Contact directly or indirectly with the elderly ranges from a single session to several years while the purpose of contact ranges from attitude change to the alteration of social distance. The multidimensional representation of intergenerational programming in the literature makes conclusive and consistent results difficult to observe. Research Findings Counselors, teachers, recreational therapists, day care supervisors, and nurses are some of the individuals involved in facilitating intergenerational programs. Counselors and teachers utilize intergenerational programs to develop social skills and positive self esteem among children and older adults. Intergenerational programs can focus on intergenerational theory, interage contact, or a combination of theory and contact. Through carefully developed intergenerational programs, teachers and counselors can address the social and emotional strands of the curriculum while positively affecting children's academic performance (Newman & Riess, 1992; Haber & Short-deGraf, 1990; North York Board of Education, 1987). In a critique of intergenerational research, Fox and Giles (1993) examined a sample of 48 studies involving four categories: (a) unmanipulated contact; (b) manipulated contact; (c) manipulated contact and classroom units on aging; and (d) units on aging without intergenerational contact. Of the 48 studies reviewed, only four studies involved school-aged children despite the theory that attitudes developed early in life remain influential throughout an individual's lifetime (Weinberger, 1979). If this is indeed correct then the examination of attitudes among this population is important and appears to be overlooked in the intergenerationalliterature. Institutionalized or less independent older 4 adults also seem to be largely absent in the intergenerationalliterature. An examination of the limited inclusion of children and the predominant presence of "healthy" older adults in the literature follows in a comprehensive review of the literature. Fox and Giles (1993) found that among those studies involving naturally occurring contact with older adults, a significant directional relationship between attitudes and intergenerational contact failed to be identified. Programs included in their analysis that involved manipulated contact with and without classroom units on aging generally demonstrated significant positive relationships between attitudes and intergenerational contact when including "healthy elderly", "independent elderly", and "senior citizens in the community" (Fox and Giles, p. 430). Those programs based upon educational units on aging that were presented in isolation generally supported positive attitudes toward older adults. The participation of "healthy" older adults in those intergenerational programs including manipulated contact and/or aging curriculums appear to reflect positive attitudes among participating children. The absence of older adult long-term care residents in the studies reviewed limit the overall results. While this population remains small within the general population, they are nonetheless an important part of the older adult community, particularly as the length of post-retirement increases. Purpose of Research The absence of research on attitude measurements of children in intergenerational programs with specific populations including those with dementia evokes the question whether positive attitudes in children can exist within such programs. In reporting positive attitudes in children, the intergenerationalliterature appears to be based on intergenerational programs that include cognitively- and physically-active older adults. The activities and interactions in the dementia program that are based on the abilities of the participating residents may perpetuate negative stereotypes of the elderly residents that may contribute to 5 the children's negative attitudes towards the elderly participants. The role of gender and children's attitudes following intergenerational programs also appears to be overlooked in much of the current literature. Finally, the limited research addressing the positive or negative perceptions of older adult participants toward children and the intergenerational ~o r leaves unanswered, the question whether older adults respond positively to participation in such programs? This study will explore the connections between children's attitudes and the characteristics of the older adult population in two intergenerational programs. In addition, the positive or negative perceptions of elderly participants in an intergenerational program toward the children and the program will be examined. This is accomplished through the definition of characteristics of the elderly populations in both intergenerational programs, examination of the children's attitudes' and elicitation of the residents' perceptions. Based upon these limitations of the literature, the research questions for this study are as follows: 1. Will significant differences between children's attitudes toward elderly residents in the general program and the dementia program be observed? 2. Will significant differences in children's attitudes be related to the number of years they have participated in the intergenerational programs? 3. Will significant differences in attitudes between male and female children in the general and dementia programs be observed? 4. Will long-term care residents provide positive or negative responses as measured by the modified Older Adult interview? 6 CHAPTER TWO REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Three assumptions have generally provided the framework upon which intergenerational programs have developed (Chapman & Neal, 1990). First is the assumption that older and younger generations have little contact in society. Second, older adults and youth have negative attitudes towards each other. The third assumption is that more positive attitudes will occur by increasing intergenerational contact. The following review of the literature examines each of these assumptions and the theoretical structure of intergenerational programs. lntergenerational Contact in Society Demographics The Canadian population has increased by over7.5 million people since 1901 and the percentage of individuals over age sixty-five has increased from five percent to ten percent (Marshall, 1980). Currently, more than eleven percent of the Canadian population is sixty-five years of age or older (Statistics Canada, 1991). Estimates suggest that by the year 2000, more than twelve percent of the American population and between twelve and eighteen percent of the Canadian population will be 64 years and older (Marshall, 1980; Serock et al., 1977). Additionally, the average age of the population is increasing (Himes, 1992). Children of advanced-aged parents, who are themselves aging, are becoming more prevalent (Seelbach, 1989; Brubaker & Brubaker, 1984) as mortality rates decline (Gee, 1987). This change is resulting in increasing numbers of three, four, and five generation families (Seelbach, 1984). Demographic adjustments in the characteristics of families and their size, including rates of marriage, mortality, and fertility , affect the type and quality of care as well as the intergenerational contact these individuals will receive as older adults. 7 Family Structure In mainstream North American society we have moved away from the traditional idea of the extended family living together (Center for Human Settlements, 1983 ). The increase in the occurrence and social acceptability of divorce in recent times has potentially negative consequences for the aged. Exchange theories of family relationships suggest that those children having little contact with a parent because of divorce may feel little responsibility for the care of that parent in old age (Himes, 1992). While the literature indicates that filial support is being provided to most older adults (Newman & Reiss, 1992; Seefeldt, 1987b; Tindale, 1986; Hook, Sohal, & Oak, 1982; Sheehan, 1978), Seelbach ( 1984) suggests that, "adult children often are physically, emotionally, or economically unable to provide long-term care" (p. 98). Therefore, a complex support system involving family and social agencies may be required to meet the needs of the increasing number of older adults in our society. Intergenerational programs may supplement emotional support received from family and agencies for older adults. Institutionalization of Older Adults With advanced age occurs an increasing susceptibility to debilitating illnesses that require increased levels of care (Masciocchi, Thomas, & Moeller, 1984). Approximately five percent of those individuals aged 65 years or older will experience long-term care in an institution while twenty to twenty-five percent of older adults will receive care through a variety of community services (Brubaker & Brubaker, 1984). These investigators as well as Seelbach (1984) further identify that families continue to support older adults within the institutional setting and provide more emotional support than in previous societies. McFarland (1985) cites research indicating the existence of a significant positive relationship between informal activities with non-familial relationships and life satisfaction in older adults. It would appear that relationships with both relatives and non-related 8 individuals may contribute to the positive well-being of institutionalized older adults as they adapt to social loss including retirement, loss of spouses, and friends. Intergenerational Contact In a study conducted by Sheehan ( 1978), twenty-three percent of his sample reported interacting with an elderly person for at least one hour during the previous year, thirty percent reported at least twelve interactions, and eighteen percent reported at least fifty-two interactions. The elderly people that children reported having the most contact with were grandparents and great-grandparents. Seefeldt (1987b) identified eighty percent of preschool children having contact with older adult relatives while Hook et al., ( 1982) reported that eighty-seven percent of institutionalized residents received visits from relatives, seventeen percent from friends, and three percent from other individuals. Seventy-five percent of older-adult child-care workers reported having contact with a family member at least once per week (Newman & Riess, 1992) and eighty percent of older adults having living children had contact with them at least once per week (Tindale, 1986). The results of Sheehan's study (1978) indicate that, "the extended family is still in existence and that intergenerational contact is occurring" (p. 572). It appears that intergenerational contact is occurring contrary to Chapman and Neal's (1990) first assumption that older and younger generations have little contact in society. Summary Changes in the structure of society and increasing stress upon families require new and creative ways of meeting human and organizational needs (Henkin & Sweeney, 1989). In today's society, intergenerational programming has become a way of, "bridging the gap between the young and old and encouraging alternate forms of social organization to supplement the family structure from which both age groups are becoming increasingly 9 withdrawn" (Haber & Short-DeGraf, 1990, p. 37). While intergenerational programming cannot replace financial and medical support from family and governmental agencies, intergenerational programs can supplement the emotional support provided to older adults in today's society. Examination and evaluation of intergenerational programs can speak to the changing population demographics in our society; in particular, increasing family mobility, increasing periods of retirement, and increasing numbers of older adults depending upon a decreasing proportion of younger adults in the work force. Intergenerational Programming Intergenerational programs incorporate aspects of several theories in their structure and organization. Activity theory and disengagement theory provide support for intergenerational contact while human development theories, in particular Erikson's psycho-social development stages (ReVille, 1989), assist in identifying the developmental needs of the younger and older generations that intergenerational programs should meet. Systems theory provides the structure for intergenerational programs to meet the program objectives and garner support from the larger community to facilitate long-term success for the program. Finally, program evaluation models provide evidence of a program's success as well as provide constructive comments upon which to alter intergenerational programs and better serve the needs of the participants. lntergenerational programs that consider activity and disengagement theories, human development theories, systems theory, and program evaluation models, are more likely to be successful for all participants involved. Activity Theory Activity theories postulate that social activities are necessary for individuals to function at optimal levels and sustain acceptable levels of psychological well being (Haber & Short-deGraf, 1990; McFarland, 1985). Haber and Short-deGraf (1990) suggest that 10 from activity theory arise three major themes: That active participation with family and social organizations should provide a sense of usefulness to the elderly, that a replacement for work and associations must be provided at retirement for successful adjustment, and the development of interests and personal contacts must be continued (p. 36). Intergenerational programming provides older adults with opportunities to interact with the younger generation in settings and activities that consider the cognitive and physical abilities of the elderly participants. Social interactions limited by institutionalization or distance from family, can be supplemented in non-competitive, positive programs where the elderly are not judged but respected. Intergenerational programs that discourage the myth of the elderly as participating in their second childhood (Seefeldt, 1987a) and where the elderly and younger persons c . 05. Since the t ratio is robust to violations of the assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance 54 (May & Masson, 1990; Glass & Hopkins, 1984), the t ratio calculated was accepted because the assumption of independence of scores was not violated. The chi-square test of homogeneity found no significant difference between score distributions; x2 (1, n =1365) =7.19, p >. 01 (formula 4.2, Appendix E). The absence of a difference between mean scores in each group as well as the absence of differences in distributions of scores allowed the student scores in the general and dementia programs to be combined for subsequent data analysis. Item Analysis Item analysis was completed for the purpose of identifying the functional adequacy of the items in the modified AASQ. Discrimination indices (formula 43, Appendix E) were calculated to measure, "the extent to which items differentiate between those persons with the highest and lowest scores on the total test" (Sax, 1989, p. 232). Difficulty levels (formula 4.4, Appendix E) of items or, "the proportion of students responding correctly to [an item]" (Sax, 1989, p.234) were also calculated (see Appendix F). Table 4 identifies that the majority of items for the modified AASQ had positive discrimination indices while the remaining items had either negative or zero discrimination indices. Of the 29 items that were positively discriminating, 19 had p values of .50 or greater while 10 had p values lower than .50. Of five items that were negatively discriminating, all had p values of .50 or greater. Six items having discrimination index values of zero had p values of .72 or greater. Gronlund and Linn (1995) suggest that well-constructed examinations will contain items with low discriminating power. Sax (1994) states that, "the number of students composing upper and lower groups on many teacher-made examinations is often so small that chance could account for at least some of these negative differences, particularly when the D value is small" (p. 233). Both Sax, and Gronlund and Linn emphasize that the 55 TABLE 4 A SUMMARY OFTHEITEMDISCRIMINATIONRESULTS OFTHEANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES OF STUDENTS QUESTIONNAIRE Discrimination Indices Items Positive Discrimination 2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 12, 15, 16, 17, 20, 21 , 23, 24, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31 , 32, 34, 35, 36, 37,38,40 Zero Discrimination 4, 13, 14, 22, 33, 39 Negative Discrimination 1, 9, 18, 19, 25 elimination of items developed to measure a specified objective functions to reduce the content validity of the examination. Those items demonstrating maximum discrimination values that exceeded item difficulty (Sax, 1990, p. 235) were examined for mis-keying or technical defects. Finding none of these present, all of the items were retained. Results of the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire Analysis of attitude differences between students in the general and dementia programs began with the calculation of means, standard deviations and skew. Appendix G indicates that students in the dementia program performed more positively on 22 of 40 questionnaire items than did students in the general program. Students in the dementia program also provided more neutral responses than did students in the general program. Examination of Table 5 indicates that the distribution of total scores in the general program was more negatively skewed than was the total scores distribution for the dementia program (equation 4.5, Appendix E). An earlier independent samples t-test indicated that 56 TABLE 5 DISTRIBUTION OF STUDENTS SCORES ON THE MODIFIED ANALYSIS OF STUDENTS QUESTIONNAIRE General Program Dementia Program Mean 28.00 30.44 Median 30 30.5 6.61 2.04 !! 20 16 skew -0.91 -0.09 mean scores did not differ significantly (p > .05) between the general and dementia programs. Gender and Number of Years in Programs The examination of gender and number of years in the programs for the modified AASQ scores provided different results. An independent samples t-test failed to identify significant gender differences, t (34) = 1.42, p >. 05. However, a second t-test identified a significant difference for the variable, number of years in the program, t (34) = 2.37, p <. 05. Students participating in the programs for two years (M = 30.39, SD = 4.17) responded significantly more positively than did those students participating for their first year (M = 27.04, SD =6.80). A phi correlation coefficient was calculated to identify the strength of the relationship between the two dichotomous variables, gender and number of years in the program (see equation 4.6, Appendix E). The correlation was not significant: U¢ (34) = .05, p > . 05.). 57 Overall Perceptions of Students in the General and Dementia Programs In response to the question, "What do you not like about visiting senior citizens at the Valleyview Lodge?", students in both the general and dementia programs provided a total of 18 responses. This indicated a small number of dislikes associated with both the activities and people involved in the intergenerational program (see Table 6). In contrast, when the students were asked the question, "What do you like about visiting senior citizens at the Valleyview Lodge?", those students in both programs provided a total of86 responses which indicated a positive opinion of the activities and people involved in the two programs (see Table 7). This value was 4.8 times greater than the frequency of negative responses. TABLE 6 FREQUENCIES FOR RESPONSES TO THE QUESTION, "WHAT DO YOU NOT UKE ABOUT VISITING SENIOR CITIZENS AT THE VALLEYVIEW LODGE?" Dementia Program General Program Activities People Activities type of snack 1 when resident buddy isn't there 2 when it's time to leave la when students don't visit People seeing residents upset 1 1 when residents physically hurt students 1 residents leave messy tables after breakfast 1 when students get scared during visits 1 when residents received gifts, students didn't 1 the appearance of some residents 1 when residents physically hurt students 1 some residents are stubborn 1 walking to the residence 5 2 6 5 a These responses were provided by students with resident partners not interviewed. Total Respanses 5 58 TABLE 7 FREQUENCIES FOR RESPONSES TO THE QUESTION, "WHAT DO YOU LIKE ABOUT VISITING SENIOR CITIZENS AT THE VALLEYVIEW LODGE?" General Program Activities Dementia Program Activities People People playing games 4 their buddies 2 talking to residents 4 visiting with residents 9 reading 3 visiting buddies 3 asking residents questions 5 helping residents 1 giving gifts 2 visiting old people 2 playing games 5 meeting people 1 eating snacks I feel comfortable around old people 1 singing 5 making new friends 1 talking with residents 1 the residents 1 doing crafts 1 resident's jokes 1 going with their class 1 don't visit their grandparents often I listening to residents' stories 2 learning from residents 1 going to the Lodge 1 residents are fun to be with 1a doing things with residents I the residents 1 singing 1 seeing their buddies 1a walking to the 1 making residents happy 3 playing games 3a meeting new residents exercising the residents 1 residents seem happy seeing students 1 visiting the Lodge 2a shaking residents' hands 1 residents aren't fussy 1 talking Ia smiling 1 residents are funny 1 eating snacks la eating snacks 1 I learning how to act I residents remember most things residents like the activities 1 Lodge 1 Total Responses 21 13 30 a These responses were provided by students with resident partners not interviewed. 22 59 Results of Older Adult Interview Perceptions of Older Adult Participants Residents in the genetal program provided a total of 619 codeable responses for questions three through seven and ten through twelve of the modified older adult interview.4 Each resident provided a mean of 4.1 responses per question (see Appendix H). Of those codeable responses provided by residents during the interview, 68.9 % were positive, 17.2 % were negative, and 13.7 % were neutral in tetms of desctibing and evaluating aspects of the genetal program. The results in Table 8 indicate that descriptive and evaluative responses contributed similarly to the total codeable responses. Table 8 also indicates that behavioral and miscellaneous responses accounted for the majority of total codeable responses while both appearance and personality responses were stated less frequently. TABLE 8 Sl.JMMARY OF CODEABIE RESPONSES FROM OlDER ADULTS IN TIIE GENERAL PROGRAM Codeable Responses Percentage Accounted For Descriptive 52.1 Evaluative 47.9 Behavioral 54.9 Appearance 5.4 Personality 3.6 Miscellaneous 35.9 4Questions 1, 2, 8, and 9 were not included in the calculation of these figures. Questions 1 and 2 functioned to assess the cognitive acuity of the older adult participants and consequently eliminate those participants unable to identify or remember the children who visited. Questions 8 and 9 were analyzed according to characteristics of interaction. 60 The majority of residents described and evaluated individual student partners and other children in the program positively in terms of behavior, personality, appearance, and miscellaneous comments throughout the interview. In response to question three, "Can you describe your buddy for me?", the majority of residents described behaviors positively and evaluated positively both personality and behaviors (see Appendix I). The residents responded similarly to question four, "Can you describe the other children that visit?". The majority of residents chose to describe the students behavior positively in response to question five, "What do you think of the children's behavior?". Residents provided largely positive descriptive and evaluative responses for questions six and seven to indicate what they liked and did not like about the visiting children. In response to question eight, "Can you describe some of the activities you participate in with the children when they visit?", 34% of responses referred to activities involving the interaction of students with each other. Sixty-six percent of responses described activities involving the interaction of students with residents. When asked question nine, the majority of residents failed to identify any activities they did not enjoy (see Appendix J). The majority of residents positively described and evaluated the behaviors of residents around them during the students' visits in response to question ten, "How do you think the other residents feel about visiting with the children?". In response to question eleven, "What do you see when the residents are visiting with the children?", the majority of residents provided positive descriptions and evaluations of the behaviors of other residents and students during visits. Finally, when asked question twelve, "Is there anything else you would like to tell me about visiting with the children?", residents positively described and evaluated the children in the program more frequently than residents did their individual student partners (see Appendix K). 61 CHAPTER AVE DISCUSSION Overview of Study Subjects As part of the Montessori curriculum, fifty Montessori students participated in two ongoing intergenerational programs with fifty-one residents in a long-term care facility. Twenty students ranging in age from six to eight years and 20 elderly residents participated in the study from the general program. Sixteen students ranging in age from eight to eleven years participated in the study from the dementia program that included elderly residents diagnosed with dementia and dementia-related illness. Three students in the general program having experienced the death of a resident partner were not included in the study. Twelve residents from the general program and residents in the dementia program who were unable to articulate responses verbally during the interview and/or were unable to recall the Montessori children did not participate in the study. Some residents in both programs demonstrated physical impairments, limited mobility, and loss of function as a consequence of illness requiring wheelchairs, hearing aids, and walking assistance devices. Program Characteristics Students participated in the intergenerational programs for one or two years depending on their year of entry into the Montessori program while residents participated for one month to two years depending upon their arrival at the residence. Students visited residents at the Valleyview Lodge on alternate weeks and participated in games, reading, singing, and sharing gifts and snacks. Students in the general program were assigned to resident "buddies" and participated in cooperative activities with their older adult partners. Students in the dementia program were not assigned to individual resident but participated 62 in activities that involved all residents. Alternate weeks of the program included examination of the aging process, construction of gifts for residents, and applied journal writing in the classroom for students. Residents in the general program prepared snacks and gifts for their student partners while residents in the dementia program did not engage in program-related activities outside of student visits. Students and residents in both programs participated in one-on-one and whole group activities during visits to the Valleyview Lodge. Results Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire The results from the modified AASQ supported in part the research questions. Significant differences between mean scores of students in the general and dementia groups were not observed. Students in the general program did not have significantly more positive attitudes than did those students in the dementia program. Males in the general and dementia programs did not have significantly more positive attitudes than did females. However, significant differences between number of years in the programs were identified. Students participating in the programs for two years had more positive attitudes than did students participating for one year. There was no relationship between the variables gender and number of years participation in the programs. Students from the general and dementia programs provided a majority of positive responses in reference to the question, "What do you like about visiting with the senior citizens at the Valleyview Lodge?". Students in both programs provided 4.8 times more positive responses versus negative responses. Playing games, visiting, asking residents questions, singing, reading to the residents, talking with residents, and giving gifts were aspects of the programs that students in the general and dementia programs reported most frequently when describing what they liked about the programs. When students were 63 asked the question, "What do you not like about visiting with the senior citizens at the Valleyview Lodge?", there were few aspects of the programs that they did not like. Modified Older Adults Interview The largely positive responses from residents in the general program support the limited research on the perceptions of older-adult intergenerational program participants. Residents identified that they loved the children in the program, they loved children in general, and visiting with students. When residents were asked to identify the activities that they participated in when the children visited, 34% of responses referred to activities involving the interaction of students with each other. Sixty-six percent of responses described activities involving the interaction of students with residents. When residents were asked to identify what they did not like about the general program, the majority identified that there were few aspects of the program that they did not like. Responses from residents in the general program to questions during the interview were consistently positive. Of the total codeable responses provided by residents, 68.9 % were positive, 17.2% negative, and 13.7% neutral. Of the positive codeable responses that described and evaluated the children and program, 52.1 %accounted for descriptive responses and 47.9 % evaluative responses. With regard to the frequency of the different types of codeable responses, most frequent were those addressing behavior. These were followed by miscellaneous, appearance and personality responses in that order. Research Results and the Intergenerational Literature The results ofthis study strongly demonstrate positive overall experiences for students in both programs and residents participating in the general program. The majority of residents in the general program and students in both programs idenfied few aspects of the program that they disliked. The negative responses provided by residents and students 64 addressed external, environmental aspects of the programs as opposed to individual participants or activities. Similar findings in the literature including Newman and Reiss (1992) and others would suggest that intergenerational programs are perceived by young and old to be generally positive and enjoyable experiences (Chapman & Neal, 1990; Haber & Short-deGraf, 1990; Keller, 1990; Seefeldt, 1989; Allred & Dobson, 1987; North York Board of Education, 1987; Seefeldt, 1987a; Seefeldt, 1987b; Cherry et al., 1985; Newman et al. , 1985; Pratt, 1984; Sheehan, 1981). Residents in both programs could be described as "unhealthy" in terms of their lack of independence and health concerns. Residents in the dementia program demonstrated less independence, mobility, activity, and cognitive ability than did residents in the general program. Based upon the literature it was expected that children would demonstrate more negative attitudes in programs that included older adults demonstrating greater degrees of declining health (Fox & Giles, 1993; Aday et al., 1991; Chapman & Neal, 1990; Keller, 1990; Seefeldt, 1987a; Caspi, 1984; Seefeldt et al., 1977b; Drake, 1957; Tuckman & Lorge, 1952). The absence of a significant difference in attitude between students in the general and dementia programs fails to support these findings in the current literature. The intergenerationalliterature identifies links between intimate intergenerational contact and children's positive attitudes (Aday et al., 1991; Chapman & Neal, 1990; Keller, 1990; Seefeldt, 1987a) as well as informal contact and older adults' positive perceptions (Drake, 1957). The absence of a significant mean difference between the attitudes of students in the general and dementia programs indicates that the nature of intergenerational contact, intimate verses casual, did not contribute to children's attitudes in this study. Residents in the general program more frequently identified those activities that engaged students and residents than those activities that engaged students with each other. This result lends support to the link between the nature of contact and older adult perceptions. 65 The exclusion of older adult participants in the dementia program from this study did not allow for examination of resident's perceptions in this program. This study identified significant differences between the number of years in the programs, lending support to the findings of the North York Board of Education (1987) and others (Seefeldt et al. , 1977b; Serock et al. , 1977). The North York Board of Education ( 1987) found that students participating in an intergenerational program for their second year reported more positive attitudes toward growing old. Students participating in the current study for a second year became more comfortable with and more accepting of residents in the long-term care facility as a result of becoming more familiar with the routines during the programs. While age was not controlled for in this study, age effects of students may have also contributed to this result. Such effects were noted in the research of Thomas and Yamamoto (1975). Limitations Study design as well as the intergenerational program structure limits the results of this study. The samples in this study do not support the identification of causal inferences. The results of this study can only be considered in the context of these two programs due to the utilization of purposive sampling of the elderly and the absence of an initial measurement to identify differences in Montessori students and non-Montessori students. Confounding variables including two different teachers, different age groups in each program, and two distinctly different older adult populations also limit these results. These specific differences in each program limit the validity of comparisons of childrens' attitudes in the general and dementia programs. By controlling for age and increasing the range of number of years of participation in subsequent intergenerational program research, the relationships between these factors and attitudes among children can be more clearly defined. The structure of the programs 66 included in this study allowed children to participate in the intergenerational programs for · their first year irrespective of age or program. For example, a student in the dementia program could potentially be the oldest among his or her peers yet be participating for his or her first year in the intergenerational program. A younger student could be participating for his or her second year in the same program. In addition, this study did not control for gender differences between older adults and children. Despite the absence of differences in attitudes between male and female students found in this study, the relationships between gender, length of time of participation, and the attitudes of children and older adult participants in intergenerational programs needs further exploration considering the trend in population demographics and its relative absence from the intergenerationalliterature. This study did not examine the relationship between children's attitudes toward older adults and the quantity or quality of intergenerational filial and non-filial contact children experienced outside the program. Sheehan (1978) and others ( Newman & Riess, 1992; Seefeldt, 1987b; Tindale, 1986; Hook et al., 1982) indicate that filial contact between children and older adults remains high and Tindale ( 1986) suggests that filial contact is normally of high quality. However, few studies examine the relationship between prior filial contact and children's attitudes toward older adults following participation in intergenerational programs. Current literature examines the perceptions of older adult participants following intergenerational experiences and prior filial and non-filial contact to a limited degree. The failure to examine prior intergenerational contact among students in both programs does not allow attitudes based upon the modified AASQ to be conclusively attributed to the students' participation in the programs. Attrition required modifications to the elderly sample in this study. The configuration of the elderly sample changed in both programs through the two-year period because of death and the progressive nature of dementia and related illness. The death of an elderly partner excluded three students in the general program from this study . The 67 structure of the dementia program resulted in no students being removed from the study as a result of a death within the elderly population or removal of residents from the program. The current study did not explore whether the death of a student's assigned "buddy" influenced the student's attitude toward older adults or his or her perceptions of the program. General program residents excluded from this study also included those residents transferred from the special care unit and residents removed from the general program due to cognitive or physical decline. The exclusion of some older adults from the general program and all older adults from the dementia program did not permit examination of their perceptions toward the children or the program. An outbreak of Fifth's disease among the children canceled the remaining visits to the Valleyview Lodge for the academic year. As a result, this study included only those residents in the general program who were able to communicate their responses verbally during the Older Adult Interview and recall the children who visited the facility. These constraints curtailed an initial plan to systematically observe the interactions between the students and the residents and monitor the nonverbal behaviors of the participants. The exclusion of older-adult participants in the dementia program did not allow for a comparison of perceptions between elderly participants in the general and dementia programs. The instrument used in this study to assess perceptions of older adult participants required the exclusion ofthose participants demonstrating cognitive disabilities or the inability to articulate responses. Intergenerational programs include older adults with a variety of physical and cognitive abilities. The use of assessment instruments that rely upon both verbal and nonverbal observations in program evaluations would allow for the participation of a larger number of older adults and more accurate representation of the perceptions of the members of this sub-population. 68 Areas for Future Research While Aday et al., (1991) and others (Seefeldt et al., 1977a; Arthur et al., 1973) indicate that resiliency of attitudes in children can endure beyond termination of intergenerational programs, more longitudinal work needs to be done to outline the direction of students' attitudes over extended periods of time and beyond the programs themselves. If attitudes developed in childhood remain influential throughout the life cycle (Aday et al., 1991; Weinberger, 1979; Jantz et al., 1977; Seefeldt et al., 1977a), longitudinal research on attitudes of children following participation in intergenerational programs needs to be conducted to explore the durability of attitudes through early adolescence, early adulthood, and later adulthood. Children's attitudes in ongoing intergenerational programs and the evolution of attitudes prior to, during, and beyond participation in such programs requires further research. Streitfield ( 1976) suggests that many residents participating in an intergenerational program with children, "were unaware of what the children's presence meant to them" (p. 102). Casual observations of students and older adults in both programs of this study and comments of residents in the general program indicate that hugging, laughing, singing, holding hands, joking, smiling, and children sitting in residents' laps and on their knees, were commonplace during the children's visits. Intergenerational programs provide opportunities for older adults to engage in activities with children that often do not require verbal communication; thereby, allowing older adults of all capabilities to participate. Assessment of attitudes of all participants requires further research of older adults and intergenerational programming including examination of the perceptions of those participants able to communicate and those not able to communicate their perceptions about intergenerational programs. As attrition among older adults, particularly residents in long-term care facilities, is a natural part of intergenerational programming, research on the attitudes of children 69 concerning the social and emotional impacts of health decline and death are important issues to explore. This study excluded those children who experienced the death of their "buddy" while others excluded those children who experienced the death of a relative (Caspi, 1984). As the average life span increases, a greater proportion of the younger generation will experience interage contact and the death of older adult relatives. It is the responsibility of intergenerational program facilitators to provide appropriate information and support regarding the aging process. To assist in this task, it will be helpful to have research information concerning the attitudes of children who experience the death of an intergenerational "buddy". Future research needs to explore the relationship between death experiences and children's attitudes towards older adults. More extensive examination of relationships between gender and attitudes toward older adults following intergenerational programs need to take place in the literature. The correct impression of many children is that the majority of older adults are female (Baggett, 1977). Current demographic information indicates that females tend to have longer average ) life spans than do males (Himes, 1992). If children are to question and challenge their own ideas about aging and the elderly, exposure to equally-represented male and female older adults during intergenerational programs is essential. As a result, the relationship between gender and childrens' attitudes toward the elderly needs to be researched further. lntergenerational programs and their incorporation into the education curriculum are important areas for future research. Current demographic statistics indicate that increasing numbers of children will experience interage contact and attitude theorists suggest that attitudes remain relatively constant through life when learned in early childhood. Intergenerational programs at the elementary school level can promote positive attitudes among children in an aging society. Further research on curriculum-integrated intergenerational programs and children's attitudes toward older adults may provide direction for future curriculum development in the education system. 70 Summary The positive perceptions of students and residents in this study support similar results in the intergenerationalliterature and the implementation of intergenerational programs for generally positive experiences among older and younger participants. The failure to find significant differences in attitudes among students in the programs of this study and the identification of children's positive and negative attitudes in some intergenerational programs leaves room for further intergenerational research (A day et al., 1991; Chapman & Neal, 1990; Allred & Dobson, 1987; North York Board of Education, 1987; Dellman-Jenkins et al., 1986; Caspi, 1984). The results of the present investigation indicate the need for additional research on gender and the attitudes of children following intergenerational programs. Further research is neccessary in the areas of assessment of older-adult perceptions through verbal and nonverbal instruments, and resiliency of children's attitUdes following intergenerational contact. As well, the effects of health declines in older adult participants upon children's attitudes require further research. The results of this study lend support to recent findings in intergenerational research as well as extend the findings of other researchers of this topic. Intergenerational programs including institutionalized older adults can provide positive experiences for young and old. As part of the responsibility of elementary counselors is to promote social-emotional development within children, intergenerational programs may facilitate positive attitudes between children and older adults. Further research and intergenerational program evaluations will refine the relationship between children's attitudes and specific features of intergenerational contact including participant characteristics, settings of programs, and program activities. 71 REFERENCES Aday, R.H., Sims, C.R., & Evans, E. (1991). Youth's attitudes toward the elderly: The qimpact of intergenerational partners. The Journal of Applied Gerontology, 10 ~ 372-384. Allred, G.B., & Dobson, J.E. (1987). Remotivation group interaction: Increasing children's contact with the elderly. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling, 21' 216-220. Arthur, G.L., Donnan, H.H., & Lair, C.V. (1973). Companionship therapy with nursing home aged. The Gerontologist, 13, 167-170. Baggett, S. (1981). Attitudinal consequences of older adult volunteers in the public school setting. Educational Gerontology, 7 ~ 21-31. Bee, H. (1992). The developing child (6th edition). New York: Harper-Collins. Bocian, K., & Newman, S. (1989). Evaluation of intergenerational programs: Why and ~ 147-163. how? Journal of Children in Contemporary Society, 20 Borg, W.R., & Gall, M.D. (1989). Educationalresearch. New York: Longman. Britton, J.O. & Britton, J.H. (1969). Discrimination of age by preschool children. Journal of Gerontology, 24 (4), 457-460. Brubaker, T.H., & Brubaker, E. (1984). Family support of older persons in the long-term care setting: Recommendations for practice. In W.H. Quinn, & G.A. Hughston (Eds.), Independent aging: Family and social systems perspectives (pp. 106-113). Rockville, MD: Aspen Systems. Caspi, A. (1984). Contact hypothesis and inter-age attitudes: A field study of cross-age contact. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47, 74-80. Center for Human Settlements. (1983). An older workforce: Legal aspects and policy implications (Occasional Paper No.28 [2]). Vancouver, Canada: University of British Columbia, Faculty of Graduate Studies. Chapman, N.J., & Neal, M.B. (1990). The effects of intergenerational experiences on adolescents and older adults. The Gerontologist, 30 ~ 825-832. 72 Cherry, D.L., Benest, F.R., Gates, B., & White, J. (1985). lntergenerational service programs: Meeting shared needs of young and old. The Gerontologist, 25, 126130. Cowgill, D.O. (1986). Aging around the world. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing. Crites, M.S. (1989). Child development and intergenerational programming. InS. Newman & S.W. Brummel (Eds.), Intergenerational programs: Imperatives, strategies, impacts and trends (pp. 33-45). New York: Haworth Press. Dellman-Jenkins, M., Lambert, D., Fruit, D., & Dinero, T. (1986). Old and young together: Effect of an educational program on preschoolers' attitudes toward older people. Childhood Education, 64, 206-212. Drake, J.T. (1957). Some factors influencing students' attitudes toward older people. Social Forces, 35, 266-271. Edwards, J.A., & Lampert, M.D. (Eds.). (1993). Talking data: Transcription and coding in discourse research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Erikson, E.H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). Toronto, Canada: W.W. Norton &Company. Fillmer, H. T. (1984). Children's descriptions of and attitudes toward the elderly. Educational Gerontology, 10, 99-107. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Fox, S. & Giles, H. (1993). Accommodating intergenerational contact: A critique and theoretical model. Journal of Aging Studies, 7 (4), 423-451. Gee, E.M. ( 1987). Historical change in the family life course of Canadian men and women. In V.W. Marshall (Ed.), Aging in Canada: Social perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 265-288). Markham, Canada: Fitzhenry & Whiteside. Glass, G., & Hopkins, K.D. (1984). Statistical methods in education and psychology. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 73 Golde, P.A.B., & Kogan, N. (1959). A sentence completion procedure for assessing attitudes toward old people. Journal of Gerontology, 14, 355-364. Government of Ontario, Canada. ( 1983 ). A guidebook for intergenerational programming. Toronto, Canada: Ministry of Tourism and Recreation. Gronlund, N. E. , & Linn, R. L. (1995). Educational tests and measurement (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Haber, E.B., & Short-DeGraf, M. (1990). Intergenerational programming for an increasingly age-segregated society. Activities, Adaptation, Aging, 14 ~ 35-52. Henkin, N.Z. , & Sweeney, S.W. (1989). Linking systems: A systems approach to intergenerational programming. InS. Newman & S.W. Brummel (Eds.), lntergenerational programs: Imperatives, strategies, impacts and trends (pp.165173}. New York: Haworth Press. Hickey, T ., Hickey, L.A. , & Kalish, R.A. (1968). Children's perceptions of the elderly. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 112, 227-235. Hill, D. (1987). Promoting intergenerational programs: Triads with youth, elderly, and students, a case example. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 10 ~ 165. Himes, C.L. ( 1992). Future caregivers: Projected family structures of older persons. Journal of Gerontology, 47 (1 ), S 17-S25. Hook, W.F., Sohal, J., & Oak, J.C. (1982). Frequency of visitation in nursing homes: Patterns of contact across boundaries of total institutions. The Gerontologist, 22, 424-428. Jantz, R.K., Seefeldt, C., Gal per, A. , & Serock, K. ( 1977). Children's attitudes toward the elderly. Social Education: A Journal of the National Council for the Social Studies, 41, 518-523. Keller, J.M. (1990). Intergenerational sharing: Teens and elderly for the arts (TEA). The Journal of Applied Gerontology, 9 (3), 312-324. Kidwell, I.J. , & Booth, A. (1977). Social distance and intergenerational relations. The Gerontologist, 17, 412-420. 74 Kite, M.E. & Johnson, B.T. (1988). Attitudes toward older and younger adults: A metaanalysis. Psychology and Aging, 3 (3), 233-244. Knox, V., Gekosk.i, W.L., & Johnson, E.A. (1986). Contact with and perceptions of the elderly. The Gerontologist, 26, 309-313. Knudsen, B. (1995, December). lntergenerational Programs. Paper presented at the meeting of the Northern Interior Recreational Therapists, Prince George, Canada. Kratochwill, T.R., & Goldman, J.A. (1973). Developmental changes in children's judgments of age. Developmental Psychology, 9 (3), 358-362. Kuehne, V. ( 1992). Older adults in intergenerational programs: What are their experiences really like? Activities, Adaptation, Aging, 16 (4), 49-67. Lane, B. ( 1964). Attitudes of youth towards the aged. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 26, 229-231. Lippa, R. A. (1990). Introduction to social psychology. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. McFarland, M.J .E. ( 1985). Activity theory: An empirical test among greater Victoria residents 55 and over. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada. Mctavish, D.G. (1971). Perceptions of old people: A review of research methodologies and findings. The Gerontologist, 11, 90-101. Marks, R., Newman, S., & Onawola, R. (1985). The development of an intergenerational service-learning program at a nursing home. The Gerontologist, 25, 130-133. Marshall, V.W. (1980). Aging in an aging society. Multiculturalism.4 (3), 6-13. 75 Masciocchi, C., Thomas, A., & Moeller, T. (1984). Support for the impaired elderly: A challenge for family care-givers. In W.H. Quinn & G.A. Hughston (Eds.), Independent aging: Family and social systems perspectives (pp. 115-133). Rockville, MD: Aspen Systems. May, R.B., & Masson , M.E.J. ( 1990). Application of statistics in behavioral research. New York: Harper & Row. Newman, S., Lyons, C.W. , & Onawola, R.S. (1985). The development of an intergenerational service-learning program at a nursing home. The Gerontologist, 25, 130-133. Newman, S. & Riess, J. (1992). Older workers in intergenerational child care. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 19 (2), 45-61. North York Board of Education ( 1987). Old and young together: Impact of intergenerational programs on students and seniors in schools operated by the North York Board of Education. North York, Canada: North York Board of Education. Palys, T .S. ( 1992). Research decisions: Quantative and qualitative perspectives. Toronto, Canada: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich Canada Inc. Pratt, F. (1984). Teaching today's kids-tomorrow's elders. Aging (346), 20-26. Reville, S. (1989). Young adulthood to old age: Looking at intergenerational possibilities from a human development perspective. In S. Newman & S.W. Brummel (Eds.), Intergenerational programs: Imperatives, strategies, impacts and trends (pp. 45-53). New York: Haworth Press. Roberto, K.A. & Scott, J.P. (1986). Equity considerations in the friendships of older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 41 (2), 241-247. Rosenthal, C.J. (1987). Aging and intergenerational relations in Canada. In V.W. Marshall (Ed.)., Aging in Canada; Social Perspective (2nd ed., pp. 311-343). Markham, Ontario, Canada. Fitzhenry & Whiteside. Sax., G. (1989). Principles of educational and psychological measurement and evaluation. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. 76 Seefeldt, C. ( 1989). Intergenerational programs-Impact on attitudes. Journal of Children in Contemporary Society, 20 (3-4), 185-194. ___. (l987a). lntergenerational programs: Making them work. Childhood Education, 64 (1), 14-18. ___. ( 1987b). The effect of preschoolers' visits to a nursing home. The Gerontologist, 27 (2), 228-232. Seefeldt, C., Bredekamp, S., Jantz, R.K., & Serock, K. (1982). How elders view children. Children Today, 11 (2), 16-21. Seefeldt, C., Jantz, R.K., Galper, A., & Serock, K. (1977a). Children's attitudes toward the elderly: Educational implications. Educational Gerontology, 2, 301-310. ___. (1977b). Using pictures to explore children's attitudes toward the elderly. The Gerontologist, 17 (6), 506--512. Seelbach, W.C. (1984). Filial responsibility and the care of aging family members. In W.H. Quinn & G.A. Hughston (Eds.), Independent aging: Family and social systems perspectives (pp.92-106). Rockville, MD: Aspen Systems. Seltzer, M.M., & Atchley, R.C. (1971). The concept of old: Changing attitudes and stereotypes. The Gerontologist, 11, 226-230. Serock, K., Seefeldt, C., Jantz, R.K., & Galper, A. (1977). As children see old folks. Today's Education, 66, 70-73. Sheehan, R. (1978). Young children's contact with the elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 36 (4), 567-574. Statistics Canada (1991). Canadian Census Analyser [Online]. Available HITP://datacenter.chass.utoronto.ca: SfRJJ/cgi-bin/censci.sh. Streitfield, E. (1976). Young and old together. Social Policy, 7, 100-102. 77 Thomas, E.C., & Yamamoto, K. (1975). Attitudes toward age: An exploration in school age children. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, §, 117-129. Tindale, J.A. (1986). Successful intergenerational family relationships: Implications for programming. Conference on Intergenerational Relationships and Programming, Guelph, Canada: University of Guelph. Tuckman, J., & Lorge, I. (1952) The effect of institutionalization on attitudes toward old people. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 47,337-344. Ventura-Merkel, C., Liederman, D.S., & Ossofsky, J. (1989). Exemplary intergenerational programs. Journal of Children in Contemporary Society, 20 (34}, 173-180. Weinberger, A. (1979). Stereotyping of the elderly: Elementary school children's responses. Research on Aging, 1 ( 1}, 112-136. 78 APPENDIX A ITEMS AND STANDARD EXPlANATIONS FOR THE MODIAED ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES OF STUDENTS QUESTIONNAIRE Question Number Question (Do you think that senior citizens ... ) Standardized Explanation Response 1 think young moms and dads aren't good parents? Yes No 2 have bad table manners? Yes No 3 are hard to look after when they are sick? Yes No 4 think that young moms and dads are good parents? Yes No 5 feel safe most of the time? Yes No 6 are bossy most of the time? Yes No 7 don't like changes in their schedules or routines? Yes No 8 usually see the happy side of things? Yes No 9 don't spend their money when they have it? Yes No 10 get upset easily? Yes No 11 are easy to care for when they are Yes No 12 don't take things that belong to other people? Yes No 13 are grouchy most of the time? Yes No 14 like to gossip, spread rumors? Yes No Schedules are what Mrs. M. puts on the chalkboard that tells you what you need to do during the day. sick? Gossip means to tell stories about other people without their permission and without knowing whether it is true or not. 79 APPENDIX A - CONTINUED Question Number Question (Do you think that senior citizens ... ) 15 think nothing good is going to happen in their life? 16 like to be served and fussed over most of the time? 17 Standardized Explanation Response Yes No Yes No are unhappy most of the time? Yes No 18 are fun to be with most of the time? Yes No 19 listen to ideas? Yes No 20 remember names well? Yes No 21 keep up with current ideas and events? Yes No 22 think bad things about people younger than themselves? Yes No 23 hardly ever get upset? Yes No 24 think that other people must take care of them? Yes No 25 like to learn new things? Yes No 26 think that their children don't take care of them? Yes No 27 are picky about their food? Yes No 28 are very stubborn? Yes No 29 are able to do things on their own and take care of themselves? Yes No They like lots of attention. Current events are things that are happening today or yesterday or last week that we read about in the newspaper or hear about on the news. Dinosaurs are old news, not current events. They are not very cooperative. 80 APPENDIX A - CONTINUED Question Number Question (Do you think that senior citizens ... ) 30 poke their noses in other people's business? 31 Standardized Explanation When people ask us things that we don't want to share or we want to keep secret. Response Yes No are helpful around the house? Yes No 32 have good table manners? Yes No 33 are unsure of themselves, don't know what to do? Yes No 34 think good things about people younger than themselves? Yes No 35 like to look after themselves? Yes No 36 are in the way? Yes No 37 are easy to get along with? Yes No 38 find ways to take care of themselves? Yes No 39 are careful about how they look? Yes No 40 talk about and do more things from the olden days instead of doing things that people do today? Yes No 81 APPENDIX B MODiflEDOLDERADULTINTERVIEW 1. How long have you been a resident at the Valleyview Lodge? 2. Who is your buddy that visits from the school? 3. Can you describe your buddy for me? 4. Can you describe the other children that visit? 5. What do you think of the children's behavior? 6. What do you like about visiting the children? 7. What do you not like about visiting the children? 8. Can you describe some of the activities you participate in with the children when they visit? 9. Are th_ere any activities that you don't enjoy participating in? 10. How do you think the other residents feel about visiting with the children? 11. What do you see when the residents are visiting with the children? 12. Is there anything else you would like to tell me about visiting with the children? 82 APPENDIX C LEITER OF INFORMED CONSENT FOR PARENTS To Whom It May Concern: As a graduate student at the University of Northern British Columbia I am studying children's attitudes toward the elderly in connection with intergenerational programs such as the program that your son/daughter has been participating in with residents of the Valleyview Lodge. This research, I believe, will provide teachers, students and activity program coordinators at long-term care facilities with information that will help them review and improve programs between children and the elderly. I have been involved in the HillsideN alleyview Lodge intergenerational programs for the past two years as a volunteer with the Valleyview Lodge. If you would like your child to be part of this research project, he/she will be asked to respond to a questionnaire identifying children's perceptions of the elderly. He/she may also be observed during their participation in the program at the Valleyview Lodge. At that time, note taking will occur. Your child's questionnaire results or program observations can be discussed with you at your request. Throughout this research project, all information collected from your child will remain confidential and accessible only to myself. Your child's identity will remain strictly confidential at all times. At your request, your child's involvement in this research project will be stopped at any time. This information may help your child's teacher and the activity coordinators at the Valleyview Lodge improve the intergenerational programs. If you would like additional information about this project, I would be glad to discuss it with you by phone (777-7777). Shannon Toronitz 83 APPENDIX C - CONTINUED I would like ...,........,..-:-:------,---------to participate in this project. (child's name) (signature of parent or guardian) (date) 84 APPENDIX D LEITER OF INFORMED CONSENT FOR HILLSIDE SCHOOL, VALLEYVIEW LODGE, AND SCHOOL DISTRICf #57 To Whom It May Concern: I have discussed the study of children's attitudes and intergenerational programming with Shannon Ableson-Toronitz as she has described it in her request for ethical approval from the University of Northern British Columbia. I give my permission for this study to be carried out with students in the classroom of _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ (name of teacher) at _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _(name of school) and with residents at _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ (name of long-term care facility). I understand that all participants are participating in this study voluntarily, and may withdraw at any time. I understand that all data will be kept strictly confidential and will remain accessible only to the researcher. I also understand that in the writing up of this project, the ideptity of all participants will be kept strictly confidential and that my participation in this study will in no way affect my employment or advancement. (signature of teacher, school district administrator or activities coordinator) (facility) (date) 85 APPENDIX E STATISTICAL FORMULAS AND FORMULA DEANITIONS IN ANALYSIS Definition of Formula Statistical Formula ( 4.1) -Xi - X2 = differenee between means of each group SX1_SX2 =standarderrorfrom two sample estimates (May & Masson, 1990) (4.2) n • = sum of all observations, Pj = observed proportion 31j = hypothesized proportion J ~ = sum total of all observations j=l (Glass & Hopkins, 1984) Rh-Rl D=--.5!1 ( Pj - l'tj )2 =difference in the observed and expected proportion for each of J categories squared ( 4.3) (Gronlund & Linn, 1995)) R T P=- (4.4) (Gronlund & Linn, 1995) 3(M -Mdn) skew = --'---==- S.J2 (May & Masson, 1990) (4.5) Rh = number of students in the high group positively responding to the item Rl = number of students in the lower group responding positively to the item !! = total number of students included in the item analysis R =number of students responding correctly to an item T = total number of students responding to the item M =mean Mdn=median SD =standard deviation 86 APPENDIX E- CONTINUED Statistical Formula rtj> = P2 - PxPy Pxqxpyqy (Glass & Hopkins, 1984) Definition of Statistical Terms (4.6) px = proportion of students scored as 1 on X (male) q x =proportion scored as 0 on X (female) pY = proportion of students scored as 1 on Y (number of years in programs) qy =proportion scored as 0 on Y (number of years in programs) pxy = proportion of students scored as 1 on both X andY. Qx and qy = 1 - Px and 1 - Py respectively 87 APPENDIX F ITEM ANALYSIS OF RESPONSES TO THE MODiflED ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES OF STUDENTS QUESTIONNAIRE Total Positive ResQQnses on the modified ~ Attitudes of Students Questionnaire of Item Higha Lowb D PValue tf t6 t8 -. t8 1.0 2e 17 t5 .11 .9t 3e 8 2 .34 .29 48 t7 t7 .0 1.0 sf t6 9 .4t .74 6e t7 t6 .06 .94 7C 9 t .50 .29 8f t7 t5 .t8 .94 ~ toe 11 -.06 .62 toe 11 3 .46 .40 11 9 t .44 .28 tze 8 6 .11 .40 13e t7 t7 .0 .97 t4e 17 17 .0 .97 tsf t5 13 .t2 .82 t6 9 8 .06 .47 17 17 t5 .11 .89 tse 16 t7 -.06 .94 88 APPENDIX F - CONTINUED Total Positive Resggnses on the modified Anal:ysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire Item Hi ha Lowb D PValue 1<)(: 16 18 -.11 .97 2rf! 10 3 .40 .37 21h 14 12 .13 .81 22e 17 17 .0 .97 23[ 12 8 .24 .59 24e 10 7 .17 .49 25 17 18 -.06 .97 26[ 15 13 .12 .06 ~ 17 13 .23 .86 2gg 17 15 .12 .97 291 9 4 .32 .42 30 17 14 .17 .86 31h 15 12 .19 .84 32 16 14 .11 .83 33[ 13 13 .0 .72 34 17 16 .06 .71 3sf 15 9 .35 .65 36 16 15 .06 .89 ~ 17 12 .29 .83 3ge 17 11 .34 .80 89 APPENDIX F- CONTINUED Total Positive ResQQnses on the Modified Analysis of Attitudes of Students Questionnaire Item Higha Lowb D P Value ~ 11 11 .0 .63 40g 6 5 .06 .33 '11_ =11. b!!=lO . '12 = 8. '\! =8. e One student provided a neutral response to this question. f Two students provided neutral responses to this question. g Three students provided neutral responses to this question. h Four students provided neutral responses to this question. iFive students provided neutral responses to this question. 90 APPENDIX G PROPORTION OF STUDENTS RESPONDING POSITIVELY AND NEUTRALLY ON THE MODIAED ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES OF STUDENTS QUESTIONNAIRE General Program Item Higha Lowb Dementia Program Total Responses Positive Neutral Highc Lowd Total Responses Positive Neutral 1 1.00 .91e .95 .05 1.00 .sse .94 .06 2 1.00 .82 .90 0.00 1.00 .7SC .88 .06 3 .33 .09 .20 0.00 .63 .13e .38 .06 4 1.00 .91 .95 0.00 .63f .sse .75 .19 5 1.00 .36 .65 0.00 .88 .63r .75 .13 6 1.00 .82e .90 .05 1.00 .88 .94 0.00 7 .67 .09 .35 0.00 .38 o.ooe .19 .06 8 1.00 .91 .95 0.00 1.00 .63f .81 .13 9 .56e .45 .50 .05 .63e .75 .69 .06 10 .67 .18 .40 0.00 .63e .13 .38 .06 11 .56 0.00 .25 0.00 .50 .13 .31 0.00 12 .33 .27 .30 0.00 .63e .38 .50 .06 13 1.00 0.00 .95 0.00 1.00 .sse .94 .06 14 1.00 .73e .85 .05 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.00 15 1.00 .73 .85 0.00 .7SC .63e .69 .06 16 .44 .55 .50 0.00 .63 .25 .44 0.00 17 1.00 .73 .85 0.00 1.00 .88 .94 0.00 18 ~ .91 .90 .05 1.00 .sse .94 .06 91 APPENDIX G -CONTINUED Dementia Program General Program Item Higha Lowb Total Responses Positive Neutral HighC Lowd Total Responses Positive Neutral 19 ~ .91 .90 .05 1.00 .sse .94 .06 20 .67 .2?C .45 .05 .50 0.00 .06 0.00 21 .78 .73e .75 .05 .88 .50g .69 . 19 22 1.00 .91e .95 .05 1.00 .88 .94 0.00 23 .78 .45 .60 0.00 .63 .38f .50 .13 24 .33 .09 .20 0.00 .88 .7SC .81 .06 25 1.00 .91 .95 0.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.00 26 1.00 .55 .75 0.00 .75f .88 .81 0.00 27 1.00 .64 .80 0.00 1.00 .7SC .88 .06 28 1.00 .91 .95 0.00 1.00 .63g .81 .19 29 .33 . 18 .25 0.00 .75 .2sh .50 .25 30 1.00 .73 .85 0.00 1.00 .75 .88 0.00 31 ~ .4sf .65 .15 .88 .sse .88 .06 32 1.00 .73 .85 0.00 .88 .7sf .81 .13 33 .67 .45 .55 0.00 .88 .38f .63 . 13 34 1.00 .73 .85 0.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.00 35 .78 .45 .60 0.00 1.00 .50f .75 .13 36 .89 .73 .80 0.00 1.00 .sse .94 .06 92 APPENDIX G - CONTINUED Dementia Program General Program Total Responses Total Responses Item Positive Neutral Positive Neutral 37 1.00 .sse .75 .05 1.00 .75 .88 0.00 38 1.00 .36 .65 0.00 1.00 .sse .94 .06 39 1.00 .73 .85 0.00 .2.se .38 .31 .06 .25e .25e .13 .25 .44 ~ .35 .05