DETERMINANTS OF MATING SUCCESS IN FEMALE TREE SWALLOWS (TACHYCINETA BICOLOR) by Lisha L. Berzins B.Sc. (Hon.) Trent University 2007 M.Sc. Trent University 2009 DISSERTATION SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY IN NATURAL RESOURCES AND ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES UNIVERSITY OF NORTHERN BRITISH COLUMBIA December 2016 © Lisha L. Berzins Abstract Females from a wide variety of taxa display elaborate ornaments and aggressive behaviours that are similar to those expressed by males. Although recent empirical investigation has demonstrated that ornamental traits and behaviours of females may function by attracting mates or signalling competitive ability when competing against conspecifics for access to mates and resources, less is known about how such traits influence the mating success of free-living female birds. For my research, I experimentally examined how variation in plumage brightness and behaviour of female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) influenced their mating success. Plumage brightness of females had no influence on investment in parental care or mating strategies of males, or the quality of social mate paired to the female. These results suggest that bright plumage is not a signal of attractiveness preferred by male tree swallows. In contrast, I report evidence that plumage brightness of female tree swallows is involved in agonistic interactions with conspecifics. Females whose plumage brightness was enhanced to signal high quality were less able to retain their nest site than females whose plumage brightness was reduced to signal low quality. This suggests that females displaying bright plumage may be challenged by conspecifics of high quality to test the quality signalled by bright plumage, and is supported by the finding that females displaying enhanced plumage brightness suffered social costs, such as delaying breeding and producing low-quality nestlings. Despite these costs, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment mated with extra-pair males that were higher quality than their social mate. Behaviour of females, manipulated by elevating testosterone (T), lowered the proportion of extra-pair offspring in the broods of T-treated females. Females manipulated so that the androgenic and estrogenic actions of T were blocked also produced fewer extra-pair ii offspring, and suggest that the androgenic and/or estrogenic actions of T influence extra-pair copulation behavior of female tree swallows. In conclusion, my results show that plumage brightness and behaviour of female tree swallows influence their mating success, and highlight the importance of studies experimentally manipulating ornamental and behavioural traits of free-living female birds prior to breeding. iii Permission to Reproduce Published manuscripts Chapter 2 is a revised version of: Berzins LL, Dawson RD (2016) Experimentally altered plumage brightness of female tree swallows: a test of the differential allocation hypothesis. Behaviour 153:525-550 and is reproduced with the permission granted from Koninklijke Brill NV (permission granted 16 November 2016) Author contributions: LLB and RDD designed the experiment. LLB performed the fieldwork. LLB analyzed the data with input from RDD. LLB wrote the manuscript with input from RDD. iv Table of Contents Abstract ................................................................................................................................... ii Permission to Reproduce .......................................................................................................iv Table of Contents ..................................................................................................................... v List of Tables ........................................................................................................................ viii List of Figures .......................................................................................................................... x Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................... xii Chapter 1: Introduction .......................................................................................................... 1 1.1. Function and evolution of ornamental traits of females ................................................. 1 1.2. Mechanisms maintaining the honesty of quality signals ................................................ 2 1.3. Personality and behavioural traits ................................................................................... 4 1.4. Study species ................................................................................................................... 5 1.5. Research objectives ......................................................................................................... 7 1.5.1. Female ornamentation as a signal of attractiveness to males ................................... 8 1.5.2. Female ornamentation as a signal assessed by conspecifics .................................... 9 1.5.3. Female ornamentation and behaviour as proximate causes of extra-pair mating ............................................................................................................................. 111 Chapter 2: Experimentally altered plumage brightness of female tree swallows: a test of the differential allocation hypothesis ........................................................................ 13 2.1. Abstract ......................................................................................................................... 13 2.2. Introduction ................................................................................................................... 13 2.3. Material and methods .................................................................................................... 17 2.3.1. Study area and general field methods .................................................................... 17 2.3.2. Parental provisioning.............................................................................................. 20 2.3.3. Spectral analysis ..................................................................................................... 20 2.3.4. Statistical analysis .................................................................................................. 22 2.4. Results ........................................................................................................................... 24 2.4.1. Parental care ........................................................................................................... 24 2.4.2. Nestling quality ...................................................................................................... 26 2.4.3. Fledging success ..................................................................................................... 27 2.5. Discussion ..................................................................................................................... 28 Chapter 3: Are there social costs of displaying bright plumage for female tree swallows? ................................................................................................................................ 39 3.1. Abstract ......................................................................................................................... 39 3.2. Introduction ................................................................................................................... 39 3.3. Material and methods .................................................................................................... 44 v 3.3.1. Study area and general field methods .................................................................... 44 3.3.2. Spectral analysis ..................................................................................................... 45 3.3.3. Nest-site quality...................................................................................................... 46 3.3.4. Statistical analysis .................................................................................................. 47 3.4. Results ......................................................................................................................... 499 3.4.1. Nest site retention ................................................................................................. 499 3.4.2. Reproductive success ............................................................................................. 49 3.5. Discussion ..................................................................................................................... 50 Chapter 4: Does bright plumage enhance extra-pair mating success of female passerines? An experiment with tree swallows ................................................................... 60 4.1. Abstract ......................................................................................................................... 60 4.2. Introduction ................................................................................................................... 61 4.3. Material and methods .................................................................................................... 65 4.3.1. Study area and general field methods .................................................................... 65 4.3.2. Spectral analysis ..................................................................................................... 67 4.3.3. Paternity analysis .................................................................................................... 68 4.3.4. Statistical analysis .................................................................................................. 70 4.4. Results ........................................................................................................................... 74 4.5. Discussion ..................................................................................................................... 76 Chapter 5: Is ornamentation of female passerines related to offspring quality? An experimental alteration of plumage brightness in female tree swallows .......................... 90 5.1. Abstract ......................................................................................................................... 90 5.2. Introduction ................................................................................................................... 91 5.3. Material and methods .................................................................................................... 95 5.3.1. Study area and general field methods .................................................................... 95 5.3.2. Molecular sexing .................................................................................................... 96 5.3.3. Statistical analysis .................................................................................................. 98 5.4. Results ......................................................................................................................... 100 5.5. Discussion ................................................................................................................... 101 Chapter 6: Experimentally altering exposure to testosterone and its estrogenic metabolites prior to breeding reduces extra-pair paternity in female tree swallows .. 1111 6.1. Abstract ..................................................................................................................... 1111 6.2. Introduction ............................................................................................................... 1122 6.3. Material and methods ................................................................................................ 1166 6.3.1. Study area and species........................................................................................ 1166 6.3.2. General field methods ........................................................................................ 1166 6.3.3. Hormone measurements ..................................................................................... 1199 6.3.4. Paternity analysis .................................................................................................. 121 vi 6.3.4. Statistical analysis .............................................................................................. 1233 6.4. Results ....................................................................................................................... 1266 6.5. Discussion ................................................................................................................... 130 Chapter 7: Synthesis ............................................................................................................ 141 8.1. References .................................................................................................................... 1555 vii List of Tables Table 2.1. Results of random intercept linear mixed models testing whether nestling size or growth differed among broods where the plumage brightness of females was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls), and where brood size was reduced by removing two nestlings or enlarged by adding two nestlings, compared to control broods (see text for more details) ........................................................... 34 Table 3.1. Predictions for whether female tree swallows retain their nest site or are usurped following a plumage brightness manipulation to reduce or enhance plumage brightness compared to controls .............................................................................................. 57 Table 3.2. The number of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced and enhanced, or remained unchanged (control), prior to breeding in 2010 and 2011, and the number of females in each treatment that retained or left their nest box, and successfully bred following manipulation. Nine females were manipulated in both years and are only represented once in the data set (see text for details)...................................................................................................................................... 58 Table 4.1. Variability of alleles for six microsatellite loci used to assign paternity in a population of tree swallows from 2010–2012 ......................................................................... 83 Table 4.2. Results of random intercept linear mixed models comparing phenotypic traits of social males paired to female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. Presented are the mean (± SE) for each phenotypic trait. Sample sizes are indicated by parentheses, and vary because individuals with feathers that were damaged or broken were not measured. See methods for details of calculating condition and plumage colour metrics .............................. 84 Table 4.3. Results of likelihood ratio tests, and general or generalized linear mixed models examining whether the plumage brightness treatment (experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged) of female tree swallows influenced female extrapair mating success (i.e., presence and proportion of extra-pair offspring) or male mating strategies (i.e., whether males gained extra-pair fertilizations, maintained within-pair paternity, and their total number of offspring sired). Presented are raw proportion means (± SE), and sample sizes are indicated by parentheses............................... 85 Table 4.4. Results of linear mixed models comparing phenotypic traits between the social and extra-pair mate of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. See methods for details of calculating condition and plumage colour metrics .................................................................. 86 Table 5.1. Results of random intercept linear mixed models comparing mass and size at day 16, and growth rates of nestling tree swallows reared in broods where female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls) ................................................................................................................................ 108 viii Table 6.1. Summary of the number of female tree swallows that received implants containing testosterone (T) or 1,4,6-androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F), or empty implants (control). ‘Initiated a clutch’ refers to the number of females that laid at least one egg. ‘Hatched eggs’ refers to the number of females that hatched at least one egg. ‘Disappeared’ refers to females who were usurped or abandoned their nesting attempt, or whose eggs were depredated................................................................... 136 ix List of Figures Figure 2.1. Reflectance spectra from the back and rump feathers of female tree swallows measured before (solid black line) and after treatment with blue permanent marker to enhanced plumage brightness (top grey line; N = 14) and silicone paste to reduce plumage brightness (bottom grey line; N = 14). Presented are the means (± SE) at every 50 nm interval from 300 – 700 nm. See methods for specific details regarding plumage manipulations ............................................................................................................ 35 Figure 2.2. Mean (± SE) feeding rates (trips/hour/nestling) of male tree swallows rearing broods where a) female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls) and b) female plumage was experimentally altered (enhanced and reduced treatments combined) or remained unchanged (control). Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment group and are given above error bars ...................................................................................... 36 Figure 2.3. Mean (± SE) feeding rates expressed as a) trips/hour/nestling and b) trips/hour by male tree swallows rearing broods where brood size was reduced by removing two nestlings, enlarged by adding two nestlings, or remained unchanged (controls). Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment group and are given above error bars........................................................................................................ 37 Figure 2.4. Mean (± SE) a) length of ninth primary flight feathers (mm) at day 16 and b) fledging success (proportion of nestlings fledged per brood) for nestling tree swallows according to brood size treatment (reduced by two nestlings, increased by two nestlings, or controls) and female plumage brightness treatment (experimentally reduced and enhanced compared to controls). Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment group and are given above error bars ....................................... 38 Figure 3.1. Mean (± SE) clutch initiation date of female tree swallows according to a) whether their plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls), and b) whether they retained or switched nest sites following treatment. Clutch initiation dates were standardized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation 1 for experimental nests separately for each year. Negative values indicate clutches initiated early, while positive values indicate clutches initiated later in the breeding season. Sample sizes are given above error bars ................................................ 59 Figure 4.1. Mean (± SE) difference in a) body condition, b) wing length, c) ninth primary length, and d) outer rectrix length between social and extra-pair mates of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. See methods for details of calculating condition. Sample sizes indicate the number of pairs in each treatment and are given above error bars........................................................................................................................................... 87 Figure 4.2. Mean (± SE) difference in a) head-bill length, b) average brightness, c) UV chroma-hue (PC1), and d) blue chroma (PC2) between social and extra-pair mates of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or x enhanced compared to controls. See methods for details of calculating plumage colour metrics. Sample sizes indicate the number of pairs in each treatment and are given above error bars ....................................................................................................................... 88 Figure 4.3. . Mean (± SE) blue chroma (PC2) of social and extra-pair mates of female tree swallows. See methods for details of calculating blue chroma (PC2). Sample sizes indicate the number of males and are given above error bars ................................................. 89 Figure 5.1. Mean (± SE) length of the combined head and bill of nestling tree swallows at day 16 in broods where female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. Sample sizes indicate the number of broods in each treatment, and are given above error bars .............................................................................. 109 Figure 5.2. Mean (± SE) growth rate constant for mass of nestling tree swallows reared in broods where female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. Sample sizes indicate the number of broods in each treatment, and are given above error bars. See Methods for calculation of growth rate constants ........ 110 Figure 6.1. Concentrations of a) androgens and b) 17β-estradiol in female tree swallows measured prior to receiving implants during pre-breeding in May and after receiving implants containing testosterone or sham implants during the nestling period in June. Black circles represent control females, white circles represent testosterone (T)-treated females, and grey circles represent females treated with 1,4,6 androstatrien3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F). Repeated measures for individuals are connected by a line, whereas individual circles indicate single measurements where repeated measurements were not available. Data points were jittered for easier interpretation .......... 137 Figure 6.2. Mean (± SE) proportion of extra-pair offspring in nests of tree swallows where females were either implanted with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F) or testosterone (T), and for controls (C; sham implanted). Sample sizes are given above error bars ............................................................................................. 138 Figure 6.3. Relationship between clutch size and initiation date (1 January = 1) for nests where female tree swallows were implanted with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (dotted line), testosterone (dashed line), or received sham implants (solid line). Data points for controls were jittered for easier interpretation .................................... 139 Figure 6.4. Mean (± SE) body mass at day 16 of nestling tree swallows raised in broods where females were treated with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F) and sham implants. Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment and are given above error bars ............................................................................... 140 xi Acknowledgements First, I would like to thank my supervisor Dr. Russell Dawson for answering questions and accepting manuscripts on Fridays! Your guidance and support at all stages of this dissertation helped me succeed. Thank you for listening to *all* my research ideas, providing opportunities for research collaborations, being an exceptional mentor, and giving me the encouragement and freedom to grow as a scientist. I thank my committee members Drs. Dezene Huber, Brent Murray, Ken Otter, Kathy Parker, and Paul Siakaluk for their valuable contributors to this dissertation, and providing me access to their laboratories and equipment. I am especially grateful to Russ and my committee for providing me with support to make me feel confident in my public speaking abilities. I also thank my collaborator for Chapter 6, Dr. Mark Shrimpton, for his help and expertise running hormone assays and for the valuable input he contributed to this chapter. I thank Drs. Erin O’Brien and Nicole Sukdeo, as well as Caitlin Pit, for their advice with various aspects of the paternity analysis, Dr. Stefanie LaZerte for discussions regarding data analyses, Dr. Kim Rosvall at Indiana University for advice on the testosterone implant construction, and Dr. Rebecca Safran for being my external examiner. I was very fortunate to have field assistants that worked very hard, and I am very grateful to Chelsea Coady, Jessica Milko, Amanda Lacika, Sarah Guest, Sophia Unger, Kourtney Scott, Sarah Sparks, Jennifer Salokannel, and Maite Amat Valero for all their help collecting data. I am forever indebted to all members of the Dawson lab (2010-2016) for all of their support, thought-provoking discussions, and trips to The Thirsty Moose. I am very grateful to Chelsea Coady, Erin O’Brien, Jeannine Randall, Aija White, Graham Fairhurst, Stefanie LaZerte, Alex Koiter, and Anne Marie Florres for their friendship, and also letting me carry on chatting about extra-pair paternity, genetics, hormone assays, or statistics. For providing me with access to their property, I am thankful to Karen and Dale Steward, the City of Prince George, Yvonne Wallace and Greg Sanders. I am grateful for all the financial support I personally received from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) and the University of Northern British Columbia. Additional funding for my dissertation was provided by NSERC, the University of Northern British Columbia, Canada Foundation for Innovation, and the British Columbia Knowledge Development Fund through grants to Dr. Russell Dawson. For their never ending love and encouragement, I thank my mom and dad. I am truly lucky to have parents that have provided so much love and support throughout my life, and enabled me to follow my dreams. I am also indebted to my husband Andrew for not letting me starve while writing my dissertation! Thank you for all your support and encouragement, and our ‘science’ discussions; it truly amazes me to hear you explain sexual selection or extra-pair paternity to our friend’s parents or people on airplanes. Finally, I thank all the birds, for it has been a privilege to examine your mating decisions. xii Chapter 1: Introduction 1.1. Function and evolution of ornamental traits of females Brightly coloured and exaggerated feathers, weaponry, and aggressive or bizarre behaviours displayed by male animals have intrigued biologists for centuries. Darwin (1871) hypothesized that sexual selection was the evolutionary process underlying the development of these secondary sexual traits, hereafter referred to as ‘ornamental traits’. Ornamental traits are hypothesized to evolve because they enhance the mating success of males expressing the most elaborate forms of these traits when competing for mates, which results in variation in reproductive success among individuals (Darwin, 1871). Sexual selection may arise by two non-mutually exclusive mechanisms: competition among males for access to mates (intrasexual selection) and mate choice where certain males are preferred as mates by females (inter-sexual selection; Andersson, 1994). Sexual selection tends to act most strongly in the sex with the greater variance in reproductive success, which is usually males (Andersson, 1994). For example, studies in avian species have demonstrated that elaborately ornamented males are successful at maintaining paternity within their own brood, as well as gaining extra-pair offspring in the broods of other females, thereby increasing their reproductive success (Albrecht et al., 2007; Balenger et al., 2009). Consequently, studies exploring the function and evolution of ornamental traits have generally focused on males, with similar studies on females being less common (Amundsen, 2000b). Females from a wide variety of species also display elaborate ornamental traits that may evolve directly by sexual selection if they provide an advantage to females during competition with conspecifics for access to mates (Johnson, 1988; Rosvall, 2008) or are preferred by males (Amundsen et al., 1997; Griggio et al., 2009; Cotton et al., 2015). In 1 contrast, traits that appear to not be preferred by males or reflect female quality, or are negatively related to offspring quality (e.g., Muma and Weatherhead, 1989; Cuervo et al., 1996; Wolf et al., 2004; reviewed in Nordeide et al., 2013), are hypothesized to evolve as a by-product of selection acting on ornamentation displayed by males (i.e., genetic correlation hypothesis, Lande, 1980). The focus of studies on male mate choice, however, may be too narrow to fully understand the evolution of ornamentation in females (Tarvin and Murphy, 2012), especially since ornamental traits, such as aggressive behaviour, may enable females to outcompete conspecifics (Rosvall, 2008), but entail costs to offspring quality (Rosvall, 2011b). Since females may compete not only for access to mates, but also other resources, some authors have proposed examining the evolution of female ornaments within the broader framework of social selection, of which sexual selection is one part (West-Eberhard, 1983; Lyon and Montgomerie, 2012; Tobias et al., 2012). Nevertheless, whether ornamental traits displayed by females are merely a genetic correlation or evolve by selection remains equivocal. 1.2. Mechanisms maintaining the honesty of quality signals Ornamental traits that evolve by sexual and/or social selection should honestly signal the quality of their bearer if they are to provide reliable information to potential mates and conspecifics. Models of honest signalling propose that physiological and social costs incurred by individuals enforce the honesty of signals and that these costs differentially affect highand low-quality individuals (reviewed in Tibbetts, 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b). Physiological costs are the costs incurred by individuals to produce and maintain elaborate ornamentation (reviewed in Tibbetts, 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b), whereas social costs, on the other hand, are those associated with the possession of elaborate ornamentation and are 2 imposed on individuals by conspecifics (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1988). Models of social cost predict that individuals dishonestly signalling high quality will be punished (i.e., incongruence hypothesis; Rohwer and Rohwer, 1978; Tibbetts and Izzo, 2010) or frequently challenged by individuals that are truly of high quality (i.e., social control hypothesis, Rohwer, 1977; Møller, 1987). Although models of social cost were originally formulated to explain status signalling during social contests (Senar, 2006), the role of social costs as a mechanism maintaining the honesty of elaborate ornamentation has been increasingly acknowledged (Tibbetts, 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b). Moreover, social interactions with conspecifics may alter an individual’s physiological state, suggesting that social and physiological costs may work together to maintain the honesty of elaborate ornamentation (reviewed in Tibbetts, 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b). For example, non-ornamented female pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca), manipulated to display a patch on their forehead and resemble ornamented females, had higher levels of blood malondialdehydes (indicating increased oxidative damage), compared to non-ornamented controls, and the level of blood malondialdehydes was similar to that observed in females naturally displaying a forehead patch (Moreno et al., 2013). Moreno et al. (2013) hypothesized that social costs suffered by females dishonestly displaying a forehead patch may have been mediated by social control. Therefore, studies testing for social control of signal honesty in female birds during the breeding season would prove valuable, as evidence for such a mechanism is scarce. Social costs that maintain the honesty of ornamentation also may result in lower reproductive success, increased energy expenditure (Kotiaho, 2001), or decreased parental care (Qvarnström, 1997). For example, enlarging the size of forehead patch displayed by male collared flycatchers (Ficedula albicollis) increased competition among males for territories, and yearling males displaying an enlarged forehead patch provisioned their 3 nestlings at a lower rate compared to control males (Qvarnström, 1997). Lower investment in parental care was hypothesized to arise because males with an enlarged forehead patch spent more time involved in competition with conspecifics compared to provisioning their nestlings (Qvarnström, 1997). Spending more time engaging in agonistic interactions with conspecifics also may reduce resources available to invest in reproduction and parental care, especially in female birds that often provide the bulk of resources for producing offspring (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995). Such trade-offs may result in poorer quality offspring, but as far as I am aware, no study has manipulated female ornamentation prior to breeding and explored the consequences of dishonest signalling on offspring quality. 1.3. Personality and behavioural traits Within populations of animals, individuals vary in the expression of behaviours, such as aggression, boldness, exploration, activity and sociability (Réale et al., 2007). Because the behaviour expressed by an individual often is consistent over time and across contexts, these consistent individual differences are commonly referred to as animal ‘personalities’ (Carere et al., 2005; Dingemanse et al., 2010; Réale et al., 2010a). Personalities are generally composed of a suite of correlated behaviours, known as a behavioural syndrome (Sih et al., 2004b; Réale et al., 2010a). For example, the aggression syndrome posits that some individuals are generally more aggressive than other individuals within a population and because of the consistency in behaviour across contexts, those individuals that are more aggressive may have increased success in competition with conspecifics, but such behaviour also may be suboptimal in the presence of predators or when providing parental care (Sih et al., 2004b). Personality has been shown to be correlated with life-history traits (Biro and Stamps, 2008; Réale et al., 2010b), as well as behaviours such as foraging, provisioning, 4 predator avoidance, social dominance, and courtship (e.g., Quinn and Cresswell, 2005; Logue et al., 2009; Cote et al., 2010; Jones and Godin, 2010; David et al., 2011; Barnett et al., 2012). Behaviours and/or life-history traits may be related if they are correlated in their expression due to pleotropic effects of genes or hormones, or influenced by previous experience (reviewed in Sih et al., 2004b). 1.4. Study species Tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are small, migratory, aerial insectivores that breed throughout the central and northern parts of North America (Winkler et al., 2011). This species is a secondary cavity nester, using pre-existing tree cavities, but will readily breed in nest boxes, making them a model species for avian ecology research (Jones, 2003). Tree swallows arrive at my study areas in north central British Columbia, Canada, during late April to early May (Dawson, unpublished data), with males arriving prior to females to compete for nest sites (Winkler et al. 2011). Females, upon arrival, will also compete with conspecifics for a male with a nest box (i.e., nest site), since nest sites are a limiting factor for female reproductive success (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985; Rosvall, 2008). Consequently, there is often a large floating population of female tree swallows that do not breed (Stutchbury and Robertson, 1985). Females that acquire a nest site frequently experience territory intrusions from conspecifics and must defend their nest to avoid take-overs (i.e., usurpation; Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). Overall, competition among female tree swallows is often so intense that it can lead to injury or death (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). Egg laying at my study areas usually begins in mid to late May (Dawson, unpublished data), and clutch-size is generally 4-7 eggs. Females incubate alone for approximately 13 5 days (Winkler et al. 2011). After the hatching of eggs, the nestling-rearing period is 18-22 days, after which nestlings fledge from the nest (Winkler et al. 2011). Both sexes of parent provision and deliver similar amounts of food to nestlings (McCarty, 2002). Tree swallows have a socially monogamous mating system and generally form a pairbond with a single member of the opposite sex, but both members of the pair may mate with individuals outside of the social pair-bond. Consequently, this species has one of the highest rates of extra-pair paternity reported, with up to 85 % of nests having at least one offspring not sired by the social father (see O’Brien and Dawson, 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016 and references therein). Empirical studies testing for benefits gained by females from extrapair mating in tree swallows have reported little evidence to suggest that females obtain highquality genes for their offspring (but see O’Brien and Dawson, 2007). Rather, studies have demonstrated support for the genetic compatibility hypothesis and suggest that extra-pair mating occurs so that females can increase the genetic diversity of their offspring (e.g., Whittingham et al., 2006; Stapleton et al., 2007; Dunn et al., 2009; Whittingham and Dunn, 2010). Evidence of increased genetic diversity is also supported by the number of extra-pair males that sire offspring within a single brood (e.g., 1-4 males; Whittingham and Dunn, 2014), and that broods of experienced females paired to genetically similar social mates have higher hatching success when sired by a greater number of extra-pair males (Whittingham and Dunn, 2010). Tree swallows are one of few North American species where second-year (SY) females (i.e., in their second year of life, but first potential breeding season) have delayed plumage maturation (Stutchbury and Robertson, 1987). As such, the dorsal plumage colour of SY females is predominantly brown, although rarely some individuals display fully iridescent blue-green structural plumage colour (Hussell, 1983). SY females are hypothesized 6 to display dull brown plumage to signal their poor competitive ability, thereby reducing intrasexual aggression from older, after-second-year (ASY) females (Coady and Dawson, 2013). ASY females and males display structural dorsal plumage colour that is bright iridescent blue-green, and the colour is produced by the physical interaction of light waves and the nanostructure (e.g., keratin, melanin, and air) of the feathers (Prum, 2006). ASY females displaying plumage that is brighter, with greater blue and ultraviolet (UV) chroma, and reflecting light maximally at shorter wavelengths (bluer hue), are considered to be more ornamented because they have greater nanostructure organization of their feathers, lay heavier eggs and fledge more offspring (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Although plumage colour of ASY females appears to reflect aspects of quality, those females that are more ornamented have been shown to have greater levels of nest parasitism, poorer immune defences, and lower hematocrit levels, and they also produce nestlings that are smaller or in poorer condition (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). In males, those with brighter plumage sire a greater number of extra-pair offspring, and as a result have greater reproductive success than duller males, suggesting that bright plumage enhances male extra-pair mating success (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016). Positive assortative mating for plumage brightness occurs, which may be due to mutual mate preference for this trait or competition for nest sites (Bitton et al. 2008). 1.5. Research objectives The objective of my dissertation was to examine how ornamental and behavioural traits displayed by female tree swallows influence their mating success. Tree swallows are an excellent study species to achieve my research objective because females aggressively compete with conspecifics for access to a male with a nest site (Rosvall, 2008) and defend 7 their nest sites from intruding conspecifics (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). The ornamental bright plumage of females appears to signal quality (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013) and potentially influence male mating preferences or intra-sexual competition (Bitton et al., 2008), but the possession of ornamental bright plumage may entail social costs that influence offspring quality (Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Social interactions resulting from the possession of bright plumage may alter the physiology of females (reviewed in Tibbets, 2014, Vitousek et al., 2014) by increasing levels of testosterone (T), and provide a mechanism to explain the high rates of extra-pair paternity reported in tree swallows (e.g., O’Brien and Dawson, 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016 and references therein), but this has never been tested experimentally. For my research, I performed experimental manipulations to alter the plumage brightness and T-mediated behaviour of female tree swallows to specifically identify: (1) whether plumage brightness of females is a signal of attractiveness to males; (2) whether plumage brightness of females is a signal assessed by conspecific females and whether females dishonestly displaying bright plumage experience social costs, such as lower reproductive success, or trade-offs with fecundity or parental care that influence nestling quality; and (3) whether plumage brightness or Tmediated behaviour influences extra-pair mating by female tree swallows. 1.5.1. Female ornamentation as a signal of attractiveness to males In many avian species, males and females show assortative mating for phenotypic traits, such as plumage colour (e.g., Jawor et al., 2003; Rowe and Weatherhead, 2011; Jacobs et al., 2015). One mechanism by which assortative mating may occur is mutual mate choice, where both sexes have a preference for mates displaying similar attractive ornamentation (Johnstone et al., 1996). Male choice of mates expressing ornamental traits may occur more 8 frequently than previously assumed (Edward and Chapman, 2011), especially in species where males invest in parental care and females vary in their quality (Johnstone et al., 1996; Kokko and Johnstone, 2002; Edward and Chapman, 2011). Mate choice can be exercised by males allocating their investment in reproduction non-randomly among females (Edward, 2015). Consequently, the attractiveness of female ornamentation may influence pairing success (Griggio et al., 2005), the quality of social or extra-pair mates, or how much parental care males invest in offspring (Burley, 1988). Although tree swallows have been reported to assortatively mate for plumage brightness (Bitton et al., 2008), it remains to be determined whether plumage brightness displayed by females is a signal of attractiveness that is preferred by males. In Chapter 2, I tested whether experimentally altering the plumage brightness of females influenced male investment in parent care and the consequences for offspring quality. In Chapter 4, I tested whether experimentally altered plumage brightness of females influenced the quality of social and/or extra-pair mates acquired, and the mating strategies (i.e., maintain within-pair paternity versus gain extra-pair fertilizations) of the females’ social mate. Collectively, these experiments allowed me to determine whether bright plumage of female tree swallows is a signal of attractiveness that is preferred by males, thereby providing evidence that positive assortative mating may occur in tree swallows as a result of male (or mutual) mate choice. 1.5.2. Female ornamentation as a signal assessed by conspecifics Ornamental traits displayed by females have been shown to function in competitive contests with conspecifics (Murphy et al., 2009b). As such, assortative mating for plumage characteristics reported in a variety of species may be the outcome of competition among females, as opposed to male mate choice (e.g., Houtman and Falls, 1994; Creighton, 2001). 9 In such cases, high-quality females, presumably with greater competitive ability, successfully outcompete conspecifics for access to high-quality territories or mates (Creighton, 2001). Since female tree swallows are limited by the number of available males with a nest site (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985), conspecific females may challenge the honesty of ornamentation displayed by females who were successful at acquiring such a valuable resource. In Chapter 3, I tested whether the plumage brightness treatment influenced a female’s ability to retain her nest box, which would suggest a role for the plumage brightness of females to function in agonistic interactions. Although female tree swallows displaying bright plumage benefit by gaining high-quality males with bright plumage as mates (Bitton et al., 2008), previous studies have shown that more ornamented females have greater levels of nest parasitism, poorer immune defences, and lower hematocrit levels, and they also produce low-quality offspring (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). This may be mediated by agonistic interactions with conspecific females, and suggest that ornamented females experience social costs as a result of the mechanisms enforcing signal honesty, such as physiological changes (Moreno et al., 2013), lower reproductive success (Kotiaho, 2001), or reduced parental care (Qvarnström, 1997). In Chapters 3 and 5, I examined whether females that retained their nest site following the plumage brightness manipulation suffered reproductive costs or produced low-quality offspring. Such costs would indicate that females displaying bright plumage experience trade-offs between ornamentation and fecundity (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995) or parental care (Qvarnström, 1997), as a result of engaging in agonistic interactions with conspecific females. Overall, the results of these chapters identify whether plumage brightness is a signal assessed by conspecific females, and whether the honesty of bright plumage displayed by females is enforced by nest-site intrusions from conspecific females. My results presented in Chapters 3 and 5 also highlight the importance 10 of considering the social costs associated with displaying elaborate ornamentation when examining the function and evolution of ornamental traits of females. 1.5.3. Female ornamentation and behaviour as proximate causes of extra-pair mating In socially monogamous species, females commonly mate with males other than their social mate, resulting in extra-pair offspring (Griffith et al., 2002). Empirical studies examining extra-pair mating have generally focused on the benefits and costs of extra-pair mating to females (reviewed in Patrick et al., 2012), but fewer studies have examined whether the ornamentation or behaviour of females underlies a female’s success at gaining extra-pair copulations (e.g., García-Vigón et al., 2008; van Oers et al., 2008; Grunst and Grunst, 2014; Jacobs et al., 2015). Ornamental traits may influence a female’s opportunity to engage in extra-pair copulations by attracting extra-pair mates (Torres and Velando, 2005) or enabling females to invade the territories of other females in search of extra-pair mates (Smith, 1988). Moreover, the attractiveness of female ornamentation may influence whether females mate with extra-pair mates that are higher or lower quality relative to their social mate, although as far as I am aware, this has never been investigated by manipulating female ornamentation. In Chapter 4, I examined whether the plumage brightness of female tree swallows influenced their opportunity to engage in extra-pair copulations and the quality of their extra-pair mate. If possessing elaborate ornamentation influences social interactions with conspecifics (e.g., Qvarnström, 1997; see above), then this may alter the physiology and behaviour of females (Vitousek et al., 2014), and potentially influence whether or not they solicit or pursue extra-pair copulations. Indeed, previous studies have shown that extra-pair paternity is correlated with behaviour, such as aggression (While et al., 2009); however, studies that have manipulated T have demonstrated that females treated with T produce fewer 11 or a similar proportion of extra-pair offspring in their broods compared to control females (García-Vigón et al., 2008; de Jong, 2013, Gerlach and Ketterson, 2013). None of these studies, however, have manipulated T by preventing its conversion to 17β-estradiol (E2). In Chapter 6, I manipulated the aggressive and sexual behaviour of females, by altering the exposure of T and its estrogenic metabolites, to examine whether these behaviours influence extra-pair copulation behaviour of female tree swallows. This experiment allowed me to discern whether extra-pair copulation behaviour is related to behavioural traits of females because they are mediated by hormones such as T and/or E2 and consequently correlated in their expression. Overall, the results of Chapter 4 and 6 provide insight on whether extra-pair paternity is mediated by the ornamentation or T-mediated behaviour of females. 12 Chapter 2: Experimentally altered plumage brightness of female tree swallows: a test of the differential allocation hypothesis 2.1. Abstract The differential allocation hypothesis posits that individuals should invest in the current reproductive attempt according to the attractiveness of their mate, but studies of allocation by males when female traits are manipulated to be more attractive are lacking. In the current study, I experimentally enhanced and reduced the plumage brightness of female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) relative to controls to examine whether males adjust investment in parental care according to female attractiveness, while simultaneously performing a brood size manipulation. Contrary to my predictions, I found no evidence that males provisioned nestlings according to the plumage brightness of females. However, nestling quality and fledging success were lowest when female plumage brightness was reduced and brood size was enlarged. This may be due to the plumage brightness treatment influencing agonistic interactions with other females, and may suggest that plumage brightness is a signal assessed by females. 2.2. Introduction It is widely recognized in a variety of taxa that females display elaborate ornaments, such as brightly coloured plumage. Two mechanisms often used to explain the presence of ornamental traits in females are a genetic linkage with selection on male traits or direct selection on female traits (Lande, 1980; Amundsen, 2000b). Direct selection on female traits can arise by male mate choice or female-female competition for mates or resources, and so may be driven by sexual and/or social selection (LeBas, 2006; Clutton-Brock, 2009; Edward and Chapman, 2011; Tobias et al., 2012). Ornamental traits may provide honest information 13 about the quality of their bearer (Zahavi, 1975; Kodric-Brown and Brown, 1984; Grafen, 1990), and an accumulating number of studies have demonstrated that ornamental traits reflect aspects of female quality (see Doutrelant et al., 2008; Gladbach et al., 2010; MartinezPadilla et al., 2011; Henderson et al., 2013). For example, some studies suggest that elaborately ornamented females transmit more resources to eggs (Midamegbe et al., 2013), have higher fecundity (Jawor et al., 2004; Cornwallis and Birkhead, 2007), provide more parental care to offspring (Linville et al., 1998; Weiss, 2006; Silva et al., 2008; García-Navas et al., 2012), or produce offspring of higher genetic quality (e.g., offspring with greater immune defences; Roulin et al., 2000; Matysioková and Remeš, 2013). As such, males may benefit from assessing the attractiveness of female ornamentation when deciding how much parental care to invest in offspring. Investing in the current reproductive attempt according to mate attractiveness is known as differential allocation (Burley, 1986; Sheldon, 2000), and is expected to occur when there is a trade-off between current and future reproduction, and when the attractiveness of one’s mate signals the reproductive value of the brood, thereby influencing this trade-off (Sheldon, 2000). Positive differential allocation occurs when mates of attractive individuals invest more in offspring, whereas negative differential allocation occurs when mates of unattractive individuals invest more in offspring (Ratikainen and Kokko, 2010). Providing greater investment in parental care when mated to an unattractive individual is a form of reproductive compensation (reviewed in Harris and Uller, 2009), but compensation is not restricted to negative differential allocation; mates may also increase investment as a compensatory mechanism when paired to an attractive individual to improve a poor situation, such as a lack of parental care from an attractive mate (Ratikainen and Kokko, 2010) or when paired to partners that are not preferred (Gowaty, 2008). 14 Overall, the differential allocation hypothesis has received empirical support in a variety of taxa (Sheldon, 2000), including birds (Horváthová et al., 2012); however, the majority of studies have focused on differential allocation by females, whereas studies assessing how female attractiveness influences male reproductive decisions are comparatively less common (Ratikainen and Kokko 2010; Edward and Chapman, 2011). Since the first experimental demonstration of differential allocation by male birds in relation to female attractiveness (Burley, 1988), general support for positive differential allocation has come from studies experimentally altering female ornamental traits to be less attractive. These studies have generally demonstrated that males provide lower parental investment in the form of brood defence or provisioning nestlings when paired to unattractive females (Roulin, 1999; Pilastro et al., 2003; Matessi et al., 2009; Mahr et al., 2012). One study found the opposite, where male provisioning increased in response to experimentally reduced ultraviolet (UV) crown reflectance of female blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus; Limbourg et al., 2013). In contrast, the only study that I am aware of that experimentally altered the phenotypic appearance of females to be more attractive reported no support for the differential allocation hypothesis; males paired to female rock sparrows (Petronia petronia) with increased breast patch size showed no difference in brood defence or nestling provisioning compared to controls (Pilastro et al., 2003). Therefore, support for positive differential allocation by males when paired to females whose phenotypic traits have been manipulated to be more attractive, as demonstrated by Burley (1988), is lacking. In the current study, my aim was to test how variation in female attractiveness influences male investment in parental care, and how investment decisions affect offspring quality and fledging success in tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Tree swallows are an appropriate species to test male differential allocation because females that are in at least 15 their second breeding season (after-second-year; ASY) display ornamented dorsal plumage that is similar to males (i.e., iridescent blue-green), and males contribute to parental care by defending territories, feeding nestlings, and performing nest sanitation (Winkler et al., 2011). Previous studies examining plumage colour in females have reported that females displaying brighter plumage with greater UV and blue chroma, and bluer hues are older, make greater investments in reproduction and fledge more offspring (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). I tested whether the perceived attractiveness of females influences male investment in parental care by experimentally enhancing and reducing the plumage brightness of females relative to controls. I chose to manipulate plumage brightness because previous studies have shown brighter female tree swallows lay heavier eggs, thereby suggesting that plumage brightness may signal female quality (Bitton et al., 2008), and positive assortative pairing occurs for plumage brightness where bright females pair with bright males, which may result from mutual mate choice or competition among females for nest sites (Bitton et al., 2008). Given that competition among females is intense (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985; Rosvall, 2008) and can lead to injury or death (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985; Robertson et al., 1986), it may be more likely that bright plumage of females is related to competitive ability. Nevertheless, ornamental traits that evolve by social or intra-sexual competition may still be exploited by males when choosing mates (LeBas, 2006) or deciding how much effort to invest in parental care. Therefore, if males invest in parental care according to female attractiveness, I predicted that males paired to females with enhanced plumage brightness would provision their brood at a higher rate and produce offspring that grew faster and were larger at fledging than males paired to females with control or reduced plumage brightness. To explore whether adjustments in feeding rates of males in response to varying plumage 16 brightness of females was contingent on demands of the brood, I also simultaneously performed a brood size manipulation. I expected to see the greatest increase in feeding rates (to brood as a whole and to each individual nestling) for males rearing enlarged broods where female plumage brightness was enhanced, whereas the greatest decrease in feeding rates was expected to occur for males rearing enlarged broods where female plumage brightness had been reduced. 2.3. Material and methods 2.3.1. Study area and general field methods I studied tree swallows breeding in nest boxes at three study areas within 30 km of each other near Prince George, BC, Canada (53oN, 123oW) from May to August in 2010 and 2011. These sites are located in areas consisting of open agricultural fields and small wetlands mixed with patches of both coniferous and deciduous trees (see Bitton et al., 2007; Dawson, 2008 for more details). In early May I visited nest boxes every 1-2 days to document the start of egg laying, at which time I began to check boxes daily until clutches were complete. Beginning 12 days after clutch completion, I checked nests daily to determine hatching date (where January 1 = 1), which was defined as the date the first egg in each nest hatched (designated day 0 of the nestling period). On day 2 of the nestling period, I captured adults in nest boxes while they were feeding nestlings. All individuals received an individually numbered aluminum leg band, and five feathers were collected from the rump, and stored in opaque envelopes at room temperature until spectral analysis (details below). Males were marked with a small white dot on their back using non-toxic acrylic paint for identification during provisioning trials (see below). I determined the age of females using dorsal plumage colour (Hussell, 1983), and 17 ASY females were allocated to treatment groups (enhanced, reduced or control plumage brightness) sequentially upon capture after determining the order of treatment randomly. Prior to manipulation, females in each treatment did not differ in their plumage characteristics (all P values > 0.88; see below for description of plumage characteristics) or hatching date (P > 0.76). I altered female plumage brightness by applying evenly to the head, nape, mantle, and rump feathers, a non-toxic permanent blue marker (Prismacolor® PM-39: True blue) to enhance brightness, or silicon paste (Mucilin®) to reduce brightness (Figure 2.1); control females were treated in the same manner, but the marker used contained water. I was confident that my treatment would produce the desired effect throughout the study since these permanent markers and silicone paste have been used previously (Ballentine and Hill, 2003; Johnsen et al., 2005; Safran et al., 2005; Liu et al., 2007), and the effects can still be detected on average 15 days after manipulation (Johnsen et al., 2005). I tested the effectiveness of my treatment in the laboratory by measuring plumage characteristics (see below for more details) before and after the application of permanent marker or silicon paste to rump and back feathers collected from females that were not part of the present study. Treatment of feathers with the blue permanent marker significantly increased the average brightness of feathers (paired t13 = -8.10, P < 0.001) without altering hue (paired t13 = -1.50, P = 0.16), whereas the application of silicon paste reduced the average feather brightness (paired t13 = 7.97, P < 0.001) and slightly shifted hue (9.07 ± 1.79 standard error (SE) nm) toward longer wavelengths (i.e., made females slightly greener; paired t13 = -5.08, P < 0.001). The phenotypes produced by my experimental manipulation fell within the natural range of plumage brightness for female tree swallows (Berzins, unpublished data). To test whether enhanced plumage remained bright throughout the study period, I exposed a subset 18 of feather samples treated with blue marker to sunlight for 15 days, and although plumage brightness had faded somewhat, feathers were still significantly brighter than they were prior to treatment (paired t8 = -3.94, P < 0.01). In total, I manipulated the plumage brightness of females from 46 nests (enhanced = 22, reduced = 24) while 24 females served as controls. Two days after hatching, I performed a brood size manipulation to determine whether male investment in parental care when paired to females that differ in attractiveness is contingent on the demand of the brood. Nests were matched for hatching date (± 1 day), and randomly assigned to a brood size treatment (enlarged, reduced, or control) independent of the plumage treatment of the female. Each nestling was uniquely marked with non-toxic markers, and two nestlings were removed from reduced broods and added to the enlarged brood size group. In addition, I also cross-fostered 1-4 nestlings among nests when possible to ensure nests contained original and cross-fostered offspring, thereby controlling for the potential genetic effects on offspring quality. For these manipulations, I weighed all nestlings using a spring balance (nearest 0.125 g) and selected only intermediate-sized nestlings that were similar in mass to transfer between nests to ensure that overall size hierarchies within nests were not altered (Dawson and Bortolotti, 2003). Following my experimental manipulation of brood size, average nestling mass of the brood on day 2 did not differ by plumage treatment (F2, 60 = 2.60, P = 0.08) or brood size treatment (F2, 60 = 1.87, P = 0.16) after controlling for hatching date (F1, 60 = 3.21, P = 0.08). The suggestion of a trend for the average mass of nestlings to differ by plumage treatment on day 2 was due to nestlings in control nests tending to be lighter (mean ± SE; 3.25 ± 0.15) than nestlings in reduced (3.66 ± 0.15) and enhanced broods (3.65 ± 0.16). Mass of nestlings was subsequently measured with a spring balance (nearest 0.125 g) from day 4 to 16 and the length of the ninth primary flight 19 feather with a ruler (nearest 0.5 mm) from day 8 to 16. I visited nests on day 22 to determine the number of young that successfully fledged. 2.3.2. Parental provisioning Investment in parental care was estimated by quantifying provisioning rates of parent birds during 40 min sessions on days 6, 8, and 10 of the nestling period. Males were distinguished from females in provisioning observations by the white dot applied to the back of males following capture earlier in the nestling period. Observations were performed between 0830 and 1900 hrs PST. For each session, the observer was situated approximately 50 m from the nest and used binoculars to document every visit each parent made to the nest box. Since parent tree swallows rarely visit the nest box without bringing food, the number of visits to the nest provides an accurate measure of food delivery (McCarty, 2002). Moreover, the load sizes of food delivered to the nests are similar between the sexes and consistent throughout the feeding period (McCarty 2002). Observations were performed at 25 boxes in 2010 and 17 boxes in 2011. 2.3.3. Spectral analysis To quantify plumage characteristics of females prior to my experimental manipulation (hereafter referred to as original plumage colour in analysis), four rump feathers were taped to a piece of cardboard in an overlapping fashion that mimicked the natural arrangement of rump feathers on the bird, following Bitton et al. (2007). Samples were then placed on a black non-reflective background and reflectance spectra were measured using a JAZ-PX spectrometer with a xenon light source (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA) and a bifurcated probe enclosed in a black holder that excluded ambient light. The probe was held 90o to the surface of the feather and three measurements were taken, 20 removing the probe between each measurement. Spectral data were recorded using SpectraSuite software (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA) and reflectance was calculated as the proportion of light reflected in 1 nm intervals between 300-700 nm relative to the reflectance of a WS-1 diffuse white standard (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA). I used the pavo package (Maia et al., 2013) for R (R Development Core Team, 2015) to smooth the spectral curves and quantify plumage characteristics that summarized the spectral curve for each individual. Specifically, I quantified measures of hue, UV chroma, blue chroma and average brightness because these plumage characteristics have been shown previously to be biologically relevant measures of quality in tree swallows (Bitton et al., 2007; Bitton and Dawson, 2008; Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Hue was calculated as the wavelength of maximum reflectance, UV and blue chroma were each calculated as the relative proportion of light reflected in the UV (300-400 nm) and blue (400512 nm) range relative to the entire spectrum (300-700 nm), and average brightness was calculated as the average amount of light reflected by the feather over the entire spectrum (300-700 nm; see Montgomerie, 2006). To reduce these plumage characteristics to individual colour scores that reflect variation in plumage colour among individuals, I used a principal components analysis (Montgomerie, 2006) using all ASY females from which I collected feather samples over a four-year period (2010-2013). The first principal component (PC1) explained 55.8 % of the total variation and the factor loadings (hue: -0.98; UV chroma: 0.88; blue chroma: 0.70; brightness: -0.07) suggest females with larger positive PC1 scores had greater UV and blue chroma, and reflected at shorter wavelengths (i.e., were bluer). PC2 explained 26.2 % of the total variation and was heavily weighted by brightness (factor loadings = 0.12, -0.18, 0.30, and 0.95, respectively), so large positive PC2 scores represent females with brighter plumage. Overall, females displaying brighter plumage with greater 21 UV and blue chroma, and bluer hues (larger, positive PC scores) are considered more ornamented because such females have been shown to make greater investments in reproduction and have greater reproductive success (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). 2.3.4. Statistical analysis To determine whether the experimental manipulation of female plumage brightness influenced male investment in parental care, I analyzed the rate of food delivered to the nest by male tree swallows using linear mixed models (LMM). Brood identity was the subject grouping variable and chick age defined the repeated effect to account for the feeding observations that occurred on days 6, 8 and 10. I was interested both in assessing how much males invested into each individual nestling and in the brood as a whole, so I ran models using the number of feeding trips per hour per nestling and number of feeding trips per hour as the dependent variables. Plumage brightness treatment and brood size treatment, as well as study area were included as fixed factors, and I tested for an interaction between plumage brightness and brood size treatments. I did not include year as a factor because it was confounded with study area. As covariates, I included the feeding rate and original plumage colour (PC1 and PC2) of the female, and the start time of the observation. Since climatic variation can influence parental feeding rates in tree swallows (Rose, 2009), I also included rain as a categorical variable to indicate whether the trial ended because it had started to rain, as well as the average temperature and wind speed for the 3 hour period prior to each feeding observation; these data were obtained from weather stations located at each study area. To test for effects of female plumage brightness and brood size treatments on offspring quality, I used LMM to examine length of the ninth primary feather and mass at 22 day 16 (just prior to fledging), as well as growth rates of nestlings. Growth rate constants of individual nestlings were calculated using a linear model for ninth primary and a logistic model for mass following Dawson et al. (2005). Growth rate constants were only calculated for nestlings that had complete growth measures, i.e., day 8-16 for ninth primary feather and day 4-16 for mass. In models, the size or growth rate of nestlings was the dependent variable, and study area, plumage brightness treatment and brood size treatment were included as fixed factors, as well as the interaction between plumage brightness and brood size treatments. Original brood size was included as a covariate for models testing size and growth, and as an additional covariate for offspring size I included the time of day when nestlings were measured; this covariate was not included in models testing nestling growth since I corrected for time when calculating growth rate constants. Hatching date was not included as a covariate in these models because it was negatively related to the time of day that nestlings were measured (r = -0.34, P = 0.01). Brood of rearing was included as a random factor to account for the lack of independence among nestlings reared within the same environment, and brood of origin as a random factor to account for any potential genetic or maternal effects on nestling quality. Due to the lack of independence among nestlings reared together, brood of rearing was always retained in the model, and I used the Wald Z statistic to assess the inclusion of brood of origin in each model (Garson, 2012). Fledging success was calculated as the proportion of nestlings that successfully fledged relative to the total number of offspring in the nest following the brood size manipulation on day 2. I tested whether fledging success differed by treatment using analysis of covariance, with proportion of offspring that fledged successfully as the dependent variable. As fixed factors, I included plumage brightness and brood size treatments, as well as study area, and tested for an interaction between plumage brightness and brood size 23 treatments. Hatching date, standardized to a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 for experimental nests within each study area, was included as a covariate. For all analyses, I removed interactions, covariates and main effects that did not approach significance (P ≥ 0.10) in a backwards, stepwise fashion, but always retained plumage brightness and brood size treatments in models. Residuals of models were checked for normality and heteroscedasticity (Cleasby and Nakagawa, 2011). Results were considered significant at P ≤ 0.05, and the overall significance of omnibus tests was examined using post-hoc tests with a Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons. Where appropriate, I report least squares means ± 1 standard error, parameter estimates (B), and effect sizes. Effects sizes were calculated as partial eta squared (ηp2) for omnibus statistical tests (Lakens, 2013) and as the correlation coefficient (r) for post-hoc comparisons (Field et al., 2012). All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v20 statistical software (IBM Corp., 2011). 2.4. Results 2.4.1. Parental care Feeding rates (trips/hour/nestling) of male tree swallows did not vary among plumage brightness treatments (F2, 95.67 =1.61, P = 0.21; Figure 2.2a) or brood size treatments (F2, 94.37 = 2.11, P = 0.13, Figure 2.3a), but increased with higher feeding rates by females (B = 0.16 ± 0.07, F1, 104.70 = 4.61, P = 0.03) and decreased as original brood size increased (B = -0.11 ± 0.05, F1, 87.81 = 4.52, P = 0.04). When wind speeds were higher prior to the observation period, males fed nestlings at a greater rate (B = 0.19 ± 0.09, F1, 85.96 = 4.07, P = 0.047). Original plumage colour of the female (PC1 and PC2), start time, temperature, and rain had no effect on male feeding rates (P values > 0.22). Performing the analysis using trips per hour showed the same overall conclusions, but feeding rates differed by brood size treatment 24 (F2, 102.00 = 3.99, P = 0.02, Figure 2.3b). Post-hoc tests showed that the number of feeding trips per hour by males was greater for enlarged versus both reduced (t97.80 = 2.56, P = 0.04, r = 0.25) and control broods (t105.13 = 2.28, P = 0.07, r = 0.22), but no difference was detected between control and reduced broods (t102.66 = 0.38, P = 1.00, r = 0.04). Although my results showed no effects of plumage treatments on provisioning by males (Figure 2.2a), I further investigated whether male behaviour was influenced simply by their mates having altered plumage brightness, regardless of whether it was reduced or enhanced. Results that compared males paired to control females with those where data from both experimental treatments were combined showed a trend for feeding rates of males to be higher in experimental than control treatments (F1, 95.00 = 3.38, P = 0.069; Figure 2.2b). Manipulated brood size, female feeding rate and wind were still significant predictors of male feeding rates (all P values < 0.046). To clarify whether the increase in male feeding rates when paired to experimental females was due to differential allocation or compensation by males, I additionally analyzed the feeding rates of females. Feeding trips per hour per nestling did not differ by plumage brightness treatment (F2, 110.18 = 0.51, P = 0.60), but varied by brood size treatment (F2, 109.76 = 13.82, P < 0.001). Post-hoc tests showed that females fed nestlings in the enlarged treatment at a lower rate than nestlings in the reduced (t109.77 = 4.94, P < 0.0001, r = 0.43) and control brood sizes (t108.80 = 4.07, P < 0.0001, r = 0.36). Female feeding rates also differed by the age of nestlings (F2, 64.57 = 5.41, P = 0.007) and study area (F1, 109.87 = 4.47, P = 0.04), and were related to the original size of the brood (B = -0.28 ± 0.06, F1, 109.82 = 20.31, P < 0.0001). When I analyzed how many times females provisioned per hour, I similarly found that female feeding rate did not differ by plumage brightness treatment (F2, 113.40 = 0.001, P > 0.99) and the number of feeding trips per hour also did not 25 differ according to brood size treatment (F2, 113.17 = 1.28, P = 0.28). Nestling age and original brood size were still significant predictors of female feeding rates (both P values < 0.004). 2.4.2. Nestling quality Analysis of length of ninth primary feathers of nestlings revealed a significant interaction between female plumage brightness and brood size treatments (Table 2.1), and so I analyzed the data separately by plumage brightness treatment. The analysis revealed that in the reduced plumage brightness treatment, the length of ninth primaries of nestlings at day 16 differed by brood size treatment (F2, 13.43 = 23.59, P < 0.0001, Figure 2.4a). Post-hoc tests showed that nestlings reared in enlarged broods had shorter ninth primary feathers compared to nestlings reared in control (t11.30 = 5.90, P < 0.0001, r = 0.87) and reduced broods (t13.38 = 6.53, P < 0.0001, r = 0.87; Figure 2.4a), but there were no differences between control and reduced broods (t16.20 = 1.36, P = 0.58, r = 0.32). Nestlings reared in nests where female plumage brightness treatment was control or experimentally enhanced had similar sized ninth primary feathers prior to fledging regardless of brood size treatment (P values > 0.51). Nestling body mass at 16 days of age was similar among plumage brightness treatments, but increased with time of day nestlings were measured (B = 0.23 ± 0.09) and varied by brood size treatment (Table 2.1). Post-hoc tests indicated that nestlings raised in broods that were experimentally reduced in size were heavier than nestlings reared in control (t51.00 = 2.75, P = 0.02, r = 0.36) and enlarged (t40.80 = 4.44, P < 0.0001, r = 0.57) broods. Nestlings raised in control broods also tended to be heavier than nestlings raised in enlarged broods (t45.38 = 2.28, P = 0.08, r = 0.32). Brood of rearing and brood of origin also predicted nestling mass among broods (covariance parameter estimate = 1.74 ± 0.50, Wald Z = 3.48, P 26 = 0.001, and covariance parameter estimate = 0.42 ± 0.24, Wald Z = 1.78, P = 0.08, respectively). Growth of nestling ninth primary feathers was not influenced by the plumage brightness treatment of the female, but differed by brood size treatment (Table 2.1). Post-hoc tests indicated that nestlings raised in enlarged broods grew ninth primary feathers slower than nestlings in reduced broods (t40.28 = -2.65, P = 0.03, r = 0.39). No differences between enlarged and control broods (t40.04 = -1.50, P = 0.42, r = 0.23) or reduced and control broods (t42.00 = 1.61, P = 0.34, r = 0.28) were detected. Nestlings gained mass at a similar rate regardless of the plumage brightness treatment of the female or brood size treatment; however, the study area influenced how fast nestlings gained body mass (Table 2.1). 2.4.3. Fledging success The proportion of nestlings that fledged per brood was similar among plumage brightness treatments (F2,58 = 0.07, P = 0.93, ηp2 = 0.002), but differed among brood size treatments (F2, 58 = 8.14, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.22) and study areas (F2, 58 = 3.62, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.11), and decreased with hatching date (B = -0.09 ± 0.04, F1,58 = 4.95, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.08); however, there was also some suggestion of an interaction between plumage brightness treatment and brood size treatment (F4, 58 = 2.29, P = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.14). Analyzing the data by plumage treatment showed that within the reduced plumage brightness treatment fledging success differed by brood size treatment (F2, 18 = 21.58, P < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.71, Figure 2.4b), but no effects of hatching date (F1, 18 = 2.79, P = 0.11, ηp2 = 0.13) or study area (F2, 18 = 0.32, P = 0.73, ηp2 = 0.03) were detected. Post-hoc tests showed that fewer nestlings fledged from enlarged broods compared to control (t18 = 5.20, P < 0.0001, r = 0.77) and reduced broods (t18 = 6.02, P < 0.0001, r = 0.82; Figure 2.4b), but no differences in fledging success were 27 detected between control and reduced broods (t18 = 1.50, P = 0.46, r = 0.33). The low proportion of fledging success for the enlarged treatment group was because 5 of 8 nests failed prior to fledging, presumably because parents abandoned their nesting attempts. In contrast, fledging success did not differ by brood size treatment in the control or enhanced plumage brightness treatments (all P values > 0.35; Figure 2.4b). Fledging success differed by study area (F2,18 = 3.82, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.30) and was negatively related to hatching date in control broods (B = -0.15 ± 0.07, F1, 16 = 4.72, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.21); these effects were not detected in broods where female plumage was experimentally enhanced (study area: F2, 16 = 1.28, P = 0.31, ηp2 = 0.14; hatching date: B = -0.04 ± 0.09, F1,16 = 0.20, P = 0.66, ηp2 = 0.01). 2.5. Discussion My aim was to test whether female attractiveness influenced male investment in parental care by experimentally altering the plumage brightness of female tree swallows. I also simultaneously manipulated brood size to determine whether male investment in parental care when mated to females that differed in attractiveness is contingent on the demand of the brood. Contrary to my predictions, I found no evidence that males adjust their feeding rates when paired to females that differed in plumage brightness (Figure 2.2a); therefore, my experiment provides no support that differential allocation occurs in tree swallows in response to variation in plumage brightness of females, although I recognize my samples sizes were small and may have limited my ability to detect differences in male feeding rates. However, when I compared feeding rates of males between all experimental broods (i.e., reduced and enhanced treatments combined) and control broods, my results showed that males tended to feed experimental broods at a higher rate than control broods (Figure 2.2b). 28 Higher feeding rates by males rearing experimental broods may be due to males in each treatment group responding differently to the change in plumage brightness of their mate. For example, males in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have increased their investment in parental care when mated to an attractive mate (positive differential allocation), as previously demonstrated by Burley (1988), whereas males rearing broods where female plumage brightness was reduced may have increased their feeding rate to compensate for the perceived poor quality of their mate (Harris and Uller, 2009). This pattern of negative differential allocation has been previously demonstrated in blue tits, where males increased their provisioning rates in response to an experimental reduction of their mate’s UV crown colour (Limbourg et al., 2013). Negative differential allocation was also recently suggested to occur in tree swallows by a study showing that both sexes provisioned at higher rates when mated to a partner with greener plumage (Dakin et al., 2016). However, it seems unlikely that my results are the response of males adjusting their level of care according to the attractiveness of female plumage brightness. Since my experiments were performed when nestlings were two days old, a more likely explanation for this trend is that males perceived the sudden change in their mates’ phenotype as an indication that something was amiss, and increased provisioning to compensate. That female feeding rates did not differ in relation to the plumage brightness treatment is consistent with the idea that males increased provisioning to compensate for the changed appearance of females and not because of lower parental investment by females with manipulated plumage brightness. Previous studies testing male differential allocation have generally demonstrated that males adjust investment in parental care according to mate attractiveness as predicted by positive differential allocation (e.g., Burley, 1988; Roulin, 1999; Pilastro et al., 2003; Matessi et al., 2009; Mahr et al., 2012, but see Limbourg et al., 2013). For example, in response to 29 reduced plumage spottiness in female barn owls (Tyto alba) and reduced UV crown reflectance in female blue tits, males provisioned nestlings less than males paired to control females (Roulin, 1999; Mahr et al., 2012). While provisioning rate of male rock sparrows was not influenced by the reduced breast patch size of females, males did reduce their level of nest defence when paired to females displaying the less attractive phenotype compared to control females (Pilastro et al., 2003; Matessi et al., 2009). That male tree swallows did not provision in relation to female attractiveness suggests that plumage brightness is not a signal assessed by males, at least when making decisions on how much to invest in parental care. Given that less ornamented female tree swallows receive less aggression from older conspecific females (Coady and Dawson, 2013), it is possible that plumage brightness of females is instead a signal assessed by conspecific females. Since males did not provision nestlings less when paired to females whose plumage brightness had been reduced and females fed nestlings at a similar rate regardless of their plumage brightness treatment, it is difficult to reconcile why nestlings reared in the treatment where female plumage brightness was reduced and brood size was enlarged grew shorter ninth primary flight feathers and were less likely to fledge (Figure 2.4a, b). Because I performed my observations during early to mid-brood rearing to ensure females retained their manipulated plumage brightness during the feeding observations, I may not have detected an effect of treatment if males or females adjusted their feeding rates later in the season. This would be reflected in the size of nestlings at day 16 and fledging success, but not provisioning rate. Alternatively, nestlings in this treatment may have performed poorly if the plumage brightness treatment affected the behaviour of manipulated females. While it is unlikely that the plumage treatment directly affected female behaviour since the application of these markers and silicone paste have been used previously with no reported adverse 30 effects on behaviour or feather quality (Ballentine and Hill, 2003; Johnsen et al., 2005; Liu et al., 2007), it is possible that the altered plumage brightness influenced social interactions among females, and the feedback females received from these interactions influenced their behaviour or physiology (reviewed in Vitousek et al., 2014b). This social mechanism has been proposed to explain the increased and decreased levels of androgens in male and female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica erythrogaster), respectively, the week following an experimental darkening of their ventral plumage (Safran et al., 2008; Vitousek et al., 2013). In my study, agonistic interactions with other females may have increased levels of the stress hormone corticosterone in females with reduced plumage brightness, a response similar to the increase in corticosterone of female rats experiencing social instability (Haller et al., 1999), which when combined with the demands of rearing an enlarged brood may have caused females to abandon their nesting attempt. Although I have no behavioural or hormonal data to support this mechanism, previous studies have demonstrated that female barn swallows with greater baseline and stress-induced corticosterone levels were more likely to abandon their clutch prior to incubation (Vitousek et al., 2014a) and broods of tree swallows were less likely to survive when corticosterone levels of females were elevated experimentally (Ouyang et al., 2015). When faced with changes in brood size, total provisioning rates (trips per hour) of males rearing reduced broods were similar to males rearing control broods; however, males rearing enlarged broods provisioned nestlings at a higher rate than males rearing control and reduced broods (Figure 2.3b). Since total provisioning rate was greater in the enlarged brood size treatment, individual nestlings received similar amounts of food in enlarged and control broods (Figure 2.3a). That per-capita feeding rates (trips per hour per nestling) did not decline with increased brood size is inconsistent with previous studies in this species (e.g., 31 Leffelaar and Robertson, 1986; Leonard et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 2000; Shutler et al., 2006; Hainstock et al., 2010). Food abundance at my study areas may be low in comparison to other populations of tree swallows (e.g., Bortolotti et al., 2011; Harriman et al., 2013), and so my results may differ from previous studies if males need to invest heavily in offspring when rearing an enlarged brood to maintain their quality. This may also explain why total provisioning rates of males rearing reduced broods were not lower compared to controls. Despite per-capita feeding rates being similar among males (Figure 2.3a), nestlings reared in enlarged broods were lighter than controls prior to fledging, which suggests that while male tree swallows were willing to increase parental investment, they were unable to fully compensate for the lower per capita feeding rates of females rearing enlarged broods. Overall, my results showed that investment in parental care by male tree swallows was influenced by factors other than female attractiveness, such as brood size (Ardia, 2007; Bortolotti et al., 2011) and weather (Rose, 2009). My results also showed that male feeding rates were positively related to female feeding rates (also see Dakin et al., 2016). This relationship may exist if males and females similarly adjust their feeding rates to the perceived needs or cues of nestlings, such as age or the number of nestlings (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1986; Ardia, 2007), or the begging intensity of hungry nestlings (Leonard and Horn, 1998; Leonard and Horn, 2001), which increases with brood size (Leonard et al., 2000; Thomas and Shutler, 2001). In addition, each member of the pair may respond directly to the level of provisioning effort exerted by their mate (Hinde, 2006). Future experiments that carefully tease apart the relative contributions of brood size, nestling cues, and partner behaviour (e.g., Hinde and Kilner, 2007) will improve our understanding of how parental investment decisions in tree swallows are determined. 32 In conclusion, I found that males did not adjust their investment in parental care in relation to the experimentally altered plumage brightness of female tree swallows. My results also showed that offspring quality and fledging success were lowest when female plumage brightness was reduced and brood size was enlarged. This may be due to social feedback females with reduced plumage brightness received following agonistic interactions with other females combined with the increased demands of rearing an enlarged brood. Collectively, the findings of my study suggest that plumage brightness of females is not a signal of attractiveness assessed by males, but may instead be a signal assessed by females. As such, selection on female plumage brightness may arise if brightness signals competitive ability, thereby influencing female-female competition for access to nest boxes (Bitton et al., 2008). Future work that experimentally manipulates ornamental traits in females prior to breeding would greatly improve our understanding of how sexual or social selection acts to promote or maintain elaborate phenotypic displays in females. 33 Table 2.1. Results of random intercept linear mixed models testing whether nestling size or growth differed among broods where the plumage brightness of females was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls), and where brood size was reduced by removing two nestlings or enlarged by adding two nestlings, compared to control broods (see text for more details). F df P Nestling size Ninth primary feather (mm) Plumage brightness treatment Brood size treatment Time of measurement Plumage brightness x brood size 1.39 1.22 4.65 3.39 2, 39.49 2, 39.88 1, 39.31 4, 39.71 0.26 0.31 0.04 0.02 Mass (g) Plumage brightness treatment Brood size treatment Time of measurement 0.03 9.99 6.53 2, 46.89 2, 45.65 1, 49.25 0.97 <0.0001 0.01 Nestling growth Ninth primary Plumage brightness treatment Brood size treatment Study area 2.14 3.58 2.96 2, 41.52 2, 40.87 2, 41.83 0.13 0.04 0.06 Mass Plumage brightness treatment Brood size treatment Study area 0.42 1.25 9.49 2, 43.65 2, 43.03 2, 43.96 0.66 0.30 <0.0001 34 Figure 2.1. Reflectance spectra from the back and rump feathers of female tree swallows measured before (solid black line) and after treatment with blue permanent marker to enhanced plumage brightness (top grey line; N = 14) and silicone paste to reduce plumage brightness (bottom grey line; N = 14). Presented are the means (± SE) at every 50 nm interval from 300 – 700 nm. See methods for specific details regarding plumage manipulations. 35 Figure 2.2. Mean (± SE) feeding rates (trips/hour/nestling) of male tree swallows rearing broods where a) female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls) and b) female plumage was experimentally altered (enhanced and reduced treatments combined) or remained unchanged (control). Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment group and are given above error bars. 36 Figure 2.3. Mean (± SE) feeding rates expressed as a) trips/hour/nestling and b) trips/hour by male tree swallows rearing broods where brood size was reduced by removing two nestlings, enlarged by adding two nestlings, or remained unchanged (controls). Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment group and are given above error bars 37 Figure 2.4. Mean (± SE) a) length of ninth primary flight feathers (mm) at day 16 and b) fledging success (proportion of nestlings fledged per brood) for nestling tree swallows according to brood size treatment (reduced by two nestlings, increased by two nestlings, or controls) and female plumage brightness treatment (experimentally reduced and enhanced compared to controls). Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment group and are given above error bars. 38 Chapter 3: Are there social costs of displaying bright plumage for female tree swallows? 3.1. Abstract The function of female ornamentation as signals of quality has gained recent empirical support; however, our understanding of the social costs that maintain signal honesty are lacking. Two hypotheses have been proposed for socially enforcing honest signals: the incongruence (punishment) and social control (like-versus-like aggression) hypotheses. In tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor), females aggressively compete for and defend nest sites (i.e., males with a nest box), and as such, nest site intrusions by conspecifics may prevent dishonest signaling. I tested whether plumage brightness of females influences nest site retention and reproductive success by experimentally enhancing and reducing the plumage brightness of female tree swallows relative to controls prior to breeding. Females in the enhanced and control plumage brightness treatments were less likely to retain their nest site than females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment. Moreover, clutch initiation date was later for females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment compared to females in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments. Overall, my results for nest site retention are consistent with the social control hypothesis. This is further supported by evidence of a social cost of delayed breeding imposed on females whose signal quality is repeatedly challenged. Since repeated agonistic interactions may be costly, studies examining the trade-offs between female ornamentation and fecundity should consider the underlying social costs that maintain signal honesty. 3.2. Introduction Females from a wide variety of taxa display elaborate ornaments that are hypothesized to evolve by sexual and/or social selection (Clutton-Brock, 2009; Tobias et al., 39 2012), or by a correlated response to selection acting directly on traits displayed by males (Lande, 1980). Competition among females for access to mates or resources is often intense (e.g., Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985; Moreno, 2015), and ornamental traits displayed by females may play a role during competitive interactions (LeBas, 2006; Clutton-Brock, 2009). Such traits may act directly as weaponry (Watson and Simmons, 2010b) and/or signal the competitive ability or status of females (Pryke, 2007; Midamegbe et al., 2013; Morales et al., 2014). Traits that function by signaling the quality of an individual to conspecific rivals should convey honest information about the signaller. Signal honesty may be maintained by the physiological costs of producing and maintaining elaborate ornaments (reviewed in Tibbetts, 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b) or social costs of possessing ornaments that are imposed on signallers by conspecifics (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1988). Two mechanisms have been hypothesized to explain how social costs can maintain signal honesty (reviewed in Senar, 2006; Tibbetts, 2014). The ‘incongruence hypothesis’ posits that low-quality individuals that dishonestly signal high quality will be punished by conspecifics through increased aggression or attacks when a mismatch between an individual’s behaviour and signal is detected during social interactions (Rohwer and Rohwer, 1978; Ferns and Hinsley, 2004; Tibbetts and Izzo, 2010). Alternatively, the ‘social control hypothesis’ predicts that individuals displaying high-quality signals frequently engage in agonistic interactions with other individuals of high quality to test the quality of signal displayed (‘like versus like’ aggression, Senar, 2006); consequently, low-quality individuals dishonestly signaling high quality will incur costs by being challenged frequently and aggressively by conspecifics that are truly high quality (Rohwer, 1977; Møller, 1987). Recently, it also has been suggested that social and physiological costs may act in concert to maintain signal honesty (Tibbetts, 2014; 40 Vitousek et al., 2014b). For example, painting the shield of male pukekos (Porphyrio porphyrio) to reduce the quality of signal displayed resulted in increased aggression from conspecifics and a dynamic reduction in the natural size of the shield following manipulation compared to controls (Dey et al., 2014). The hypotheses for the social enforcement of quality signals were originally formulated and studied in the context of status signaling during social contests among conspecifics (Senar, 2006); however, it is becoming increasingly recognized that social costs may play an important role in maintaining the honesty of ornamentation (reviewed in Tibbetts, 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b). Recent empirical studies have demonstrated that social interactions among conspecifics may alter the physiological state, such as androgen levels or oxidative stress, of individuals displaying dishonest signals (e.g., Safran et al., 2008; Vitousek et al., 2013). Such changes in physiology of dishonest signallers may be mediated by conspecifics challenging the quality of signal displayed; however, whether or not signals are tested may depend on the value of the resource relative to the costs of conflict (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1988; Tibbetts, 2008). For example, Vitousek et al. (2016) hypothesized that one mechanism explaining the difference in physiological state between male and female North American barn swallows (Hirundo rustica erythrogaster) following signal enhancement (higher and lower oxidative stress, respectively) was that signals displayed by males may be tested, while those of females may be trusted. Since barn swallows build their own nest, females may be more likely to trust signals of quality compared to obligate cavitynesting species that require existing nest sites to breed, such as pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) that compete aggressively with conspecific females because nest sites are limited (Moreno, 2015). In line with this, Moreno et al. (2013) reported that non-ornamented female pied flycatchers manipulated to display a forehead patch had higher oxidative damage 41 compared to control females and hypothesized that competition for limited nest sites may enforce the honesty of female ornamentation. Territorial defence by resident female birds toward decoy intruders is strongly influenced by the ornamentation of both the resident and intruding female (Midamegbe et al., 2013; Morales et al., 2014), which may suggest that nest site intrusions by conspecific females provides a mechanism to enforce the honesty of quality signals, but to my knowledge this has never been experimentally tested in free-living female birds. In the current study, my aim was to examine whether bright plumage of female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) is a signal of quality enforced by agonistic interactions with conspecifics. Female tree swallows aggressively compete with conspecific females for access to a male with a nest site (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985), and females that are more aggressive are more likely to acquire nest sites when their availability is experimentally reduced (Rosvall, 2008). Because some females are competitively excluded from breeding, there often is a large population of floater females that breed only when a nest site becomes available (Stutchbury and Robertson, 1985). Females successful at acquiring a nest site frequently experience territory intrusions from conspecifics and must aggressively defend their nest to avoid usurpations (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). Overall, competition among female tree swallows is often so intense that it can lead to injury or death (e.g., Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). Dorsal plumage colour of female tree swallows is variable ranging from dull brown to bright iridescent blue-green, the latter being similar to plumage displayed by males. Females displaying dull brown plumage are generally in their first breeding season (second year of life: SY; Hussell, 1983), and this delay in maturation of their plumage may signal low competitive ability since SY females receive less intra-sexual aggression from ornamented 42 after-second-year (ASY) females (Coady and Dawson, 2013). Among ASY females, those that are more ornamented display plumage that is brighter, with greater UV and blue chroma, and reflects light maximally at shorter wavelengths (i.e., bluer hue; Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Although it is currently unknown whether bright plumage of ASY females similarly functions as a status signal, females that display bright plumage assortatively mate with bright males (Bitton et al. 2008), and since bright plumage of females does not appear to be a signal of attractiveness preferred by males (Berzins and Dawson, 2016; Chapter 4), assortative mating in this species likely occurs because females with bright plumage have greater competitive ability. I tested whether plumage brightness of females influenced nest site retention by experimentally enhancing and reducing the plumage brightness of female tree swallows prior to breeding relative to controls (see Berzins and Dawson, 2016). If plumage brightness is a signal of quality that is socially enforced by conspecifics challenging females with incongruent signals, I predicted that females whose ornamentation was enhanced and reduced, so that their quality and behaviour mismatched, would be less likely to retain their nests (Table 3.1). In contrast, if the honesty of quality signals is enforced by social control whereby conspecific females aggressively challenge females displaying ornamentation of similar quality, I predicted that females whose plumage brightness was enhanced to signal higher quality than their true competitive ability would be less able to retain their nest sites, whereas females displaying reduced plumage brightness whose quality of signal is lower than their true competitive ability would be better able to defend and retain their nests (Table 3.1). It is also possible that feedback from agonistic interactions could alter hormone levels of females so that the behaviour of females becomes congruent with their manipulated plumage signals (Vitousek et al., 2013, 2014b). If this were the case, I predicted that females 43 displaying enhanced plumage brightness would be more likely to retain their nest sites, whereas reduced females may be more likely to be usurped from their nests (Table 3.1). I also tested whether the quality of nest site occupied influences retention and the reproductive success of females, since social costs imposed on dishonest signallers may result in lower reproductive success (Kotiaho, 2001). 3.3. Material and methods 3.3.1. Study area and general field methods I studied tree swallows breeding in nest boxes near Prince George BC, Canada (53ºN, 123ºW) from May to August in 2010 and 2011. The study area consisted of open agricultural areas intermixed with small wetlands and patches of coniferous and deciduous trees (see Dawson et al. 2005 for more details). Shortly after tree swallows arrived in my study area in early May, I began to check nest boxes daily to document nest building. Once a nest box contained a nest with a fully formed cup, but before any eggs had been laid, I captured the resident female. Each female was banded with an individually numbered aluminum leg band and weighed using a spring balance (nearest 0.25 g). Five rump feathers were collected for spectral analysis (details below). I determined the age of females using dorsal plumage colour (Hussell 1983), and ASY females were sequentially allocated to the enhanced, reduced, or control plumage brightness treatments by capture order, after determining a treatment order randomly. Details of the plumage brightness manipulation are described in Berzins and Dawson (2016). Briefly, plumage brightness was enhanced by applying nontoxic permanent blue marker, reduced by applying silicon paste evenly to the dorsal feathers of females, or remained unchanged by treating females with a marker containing water. Permanent markers and silicone paste have previously been used to alter plumage colour 44 (e.g., Johnsen et al., 2005; Safran et al., 2008) and since the effects of these treatments have been detected on average for 15 days after being applied to feathers (Johnsen et al., 2005), I was confident that my treatment produced the desired effect throughout the experimental period. The brightness of plumage following the manipulation fell within the natural range of plumage brightness for female tree swallows (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). Females in each treatment did not differ by age, body mass, plumage characteristics (see below), or capture date prior to experimental manipulation (all P values > 0.29). In total, I enhanced the plumage brightness of 30 females, reduced the plumage brightness of 31 females, and 28 females were in the control group. To examine whether experimentally altering female plumage brightness prior to breeding influenced reproductive success, I monitored all nests at my study area daily to record the date of clutch initiation (where January 1 = 1) and clutch size. Freshly laid eggs were numbered with a non-toxic marker for identification and weighed with a digital scale (nearest 0.01 g). I captured all females in my study area after their eggs had hatched and recorded the box and band number to determine whether females retained, left and/or switched nest sites after being manipulated. 3.3.2. Spectral analysis The spectral analysis for females used in this study is described in Berzins and Dawson (2016). Briefly, I quantified plumage characteristics of female tree swallows by measuring the reflectance of feathers using a JAZ-PX spectrometer with a xenon light source (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA). Four rump feathers were taped to a piece of cardboard so that they resembled the natural arrangement of rump feathers on a bird following Bitton et al. (2008). Feather samples were placed on a black non-reflective surface and spectral data were 45 recorded using SpectraSuite software (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA). Three measurements for each bird were taken, moving the probe between each measurement. The R package pavo (Maia et al., 2013) was used to smooth spectral curves and quantify plumage characteristics, specifically hue, ultraviolet (UV) and blue chroma, and average brightness, as these variables have been shown previously to reflect aspects of quality in female tree swallows (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). The three measures of each plumage characteristic were averaged, and then a principal components analysis (PCA) was used to reduce each bird’s plumage characteristics to a single colour score (Montgomerie, 2006). PCA was performed using all ASY females from which I collected feather samples over a four-year period (2010-2013) at my study area. The first principal component (PC1) explained 55 % of the total variation and the factor loadings suggested that PC1 was heavily weighted by hue (-0.98), UV chroma (0.88), and blue chroma (0.70) and so females with larger positive values had plumage with greater UV and blue chroma, and reflected light at shorter wavelengths (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). The second principal component (PC2) explained 26.2 % of the total variation and the factor loadings suggested that PC2 was heavily weighted by brightness (0.95) so females with larger positive values had brighter plumage (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). 3.3.3. Nest-site quality Following Potti and Montalvo (1991), I calculated ‘nest-site quality’ as the number of years a nest site was occupied in relation to the number of years the nest site was available for breeding by tree swallows. My study area was established in 2002, so I used occupancy data from 2003 to 2009 in my calculations. I confirmed whether this measure of occupancy reflected nest-site quality in tree swallows by testing whether the number of years a nest site 46 was occupied was related to average clutch initiation date at that site (standardized so that day 1 represented the first clutch initiation within each year at the site) and the total number of nestlings fledged from 2004 to 2006 (I restricted my analysis to these years as experimental manipulations in other years may have influenced fledging success; e.g., Dawson et al. 2005). Overall, box occupancy was negatively correlated with clutch initiation date (Spearman rank correlation: rs = - 0.27, N = 118 nests, P < 0.01) and positively correlated to the number of nestling fledged (Spearman rank correlation: rs = 0.57, N = 100 nests, P < 0.0001). That nest boxes used more frequently are bred in earlier and have a greater number of nestlings fledged indicates that occupancy provides a reliable proxy for nest-site quality. Nest-site quality did not differ among treatments prior to manipulation (Kruskal-Wallis analysis of variance: H = 0.49, df = 2, P = 0.78). 3.3.4. Statistical analysis I first tested whether the plumage brightness treatment of female tree swallows influenced nest site retention using a logistic regression with nest retention (retained, left) as the binary dependent variable, plumage brightness treatment as a fixed factor and quality of the nest site and original plumage colour (PC1 and PC2) as covariates; however, these covariates were not important predictors of whether a female stayed or left her nest site (all P values > 0.26), so a likelihood ratio test was subsequently performed. Nine females were captured in both years and I randomly chose data either from 2010 or 2011 to use in analyses to avoid the inclusion of multiple non-independent observations for females. For those females that switched nest sites after being manipulated, I tested whether the quality of nest site a female subsequently occupied differed before and after moving using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). In this analysis, I included time (before and after 47 moving) as the within-subject factor and plumage brightness treatment as the betweensubject factor, and the interaction between time and plumage brightness treatment. I tested whether altering the plumage brightness of females influenced their reproductive success using linear mixed models (LMM; lme4; Bates et al., 2015) that included female identity as a random factor to account for the multiple observations of females. I tested whether clutch initiation date, standardized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 for experimental nests in each year, differed by plumage brightness treatment. In this model, I also included whether females retained or switched nest sites as a categorical variable, and female body mass as a covariate. I also tested whether clutch size and average egg mass differed by the plumage brightness treatment of the female. I included year as a categorical variable and standardized clutch initiation date as a covariate to control for the seasonal decline in clutch size that occurs in tree swallows (Winkler et al., 2014). For these analyses, I used the R package lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al. 2016) to calculate P values for fixed factors and covariates. For all analyses, variables that did not approach significance (P ≥ 0.10) were removed in a backwards, stepwise fashion. I checked the residuals of models for normality and heteroscedasticity (Cleasby and Nakagawa, 2011). All statistical tests were two-tailed and results were considered significant at P ≤ 0.05. The overall significance of omnibus tests was examined using post-hoc tests with false discovery rate adjustment for multiple comparisons (Verhoeven et al., 2005). Where appropriate, I report least squares means ± 1 standard error (SE), parameter estimates (B), and effect sizes. Effects sizes are reported as general eta squared (ηG2) for omnibus statistical tests (Lakens, 2013) and the correlation coefficient (r) for post-hoc comparisons (Field et al. 2012). All statistical analyses were performed using 48 SPSS v20 (IBM Corp. 2011) and R v3.2.3 (R Development Core Team 2015) statistical software. 3.4. Results 3.4.1. Nest site retention Plumage brightness treatment influenced whether female tree swallows retained or left their nest site (G2 = 7.67, N = 80, P = 0.02, Table 3.2). To further examine these results, I performed sub-analyses between pairs of treatment groups and found that females with enhanced plumage brightness were more likely to leave their nest site following manipulation than females with reduced plumage brightness (G1 = 7.35, N = 53, P < 0.01), but not the control treatment (G1 = 0.67, N = 54, P = 0.41). Control females also tended to be more likely to leave their nest sites than females whose plumage was experimentally reduced (G1 = 3.69, N = 53, P = 0.055). In total, 92 % of reduced, 70 % of control, and 56 % of enhanced females bred following plumage brightness treatment (Table 3.2). For females that switched nest sites following manipulation, plumage brightness treatment did not influence the quality of the new nest site occupied (F2, 13 = 0.53, P = 0.78, ηG2 = 0.03); however, the newly acquired nest site was overall of lower quality than the nest site occupied by the female prior to manipulation (F1, 15 = 5.01, P = 0.04, ηG2 = 0.11). 3.4.2. Reproductive success Clutch initiation date differed by the plumage brightness treatment of the female (F2, 60.00 = 3.80, P = 0.03, Figure 3.1a), and post-hoc tests revealed that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment initiated clutches later than control females (B = -0.70 ± 0.28, t60.0 = -2.50, P = 0.03, r = 0.31) and females with experimentally reduced plumage brightness 49 (B = -0.63 ± 0.26, t60.00 = -2.37, P = 0.03, r = 0.29). Females that switched nest sites after being manipulated also initiated clutches later than females that retained their nest site (B = -0.88 ± 0.25, F1, 60.00 = 12.40, P < 0.001, Figure 3.1b), as did those females that had lower body mass when captured pre-breeding (B = -0.13 ± 0.07, F1, 60.00 = 3.57, P = 0.06). As expected, there was a significant decline in clutch size with later dates of clutch initiation (B = -0.36 ± 0.12, F1, 60.91 = 9.41, P < 0.01), as well as a decline in average egg mass with initiation date (B = -0.06 ± 0.02, F1, 47.91 = 9.58, P < 0.01). Neither clutch size nor average egg mass differed by the plumage brightness treatment of the female (F2, 60.68 = 0.42, P = 0.66; F2, 50.74 = 0.23, P = 0.80, respectively), but clutches were smaller in 2011 than 2010 (B = -0.52 ± 0.23, F1, 58.50 = 5.39, P = 0.02). 3.5. Discussion I manipulated the plumage brightness of female tree swallows to test whether plumage brightness treatment influenced a female’s ability to retain her nest site, and whether plumage brightness is a signal of quality that is socially enforced by conspecifics. Females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment were less likely to retain their nest site and breed than females with experimentally reduced plumage brightness (Table 3.2). A similar trend was also observed for control females compared to those with reduced plumage brightness (Table 3.2). There are three possibilities to explain why females manipulated to display bright, presumably high-quality, plumage ornamentation were less likely to retain their nest site than females with dull, low-quality plumage. First, lower nest site retention may have occurred if females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment were depredated as a result of increasing the conspicuousness of their plumage (Huhta et al., 2003). Indeed, of the females that did not retain their nest site and breed after being manipulated, only two were 50 observed breeding at my study area in subsequent years (Berzins and Dawson, unpublished data). If my results for nest site retention were due to predation, then females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment should have been less likely to retain their nest site than control females, but there were no difference between the enhanced and control treatments (Table 3.2). Second, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have left their nest site to obtain a higher quality site or mate (Otter and Ratcliffe, 1996) after being manipulated to signal high quality, but I find it unlikely that a female would relinquish a limited resource essential for breeding after intense competition with conspecifics (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985; Rosvall, 2008). This explanation also does not explain why control females also tended to be more likely to leave their nest site than females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment. Moreover, females that switched nest sites bred later (Figure 3.1b) and in poorer quality sites than the nest site they originally occupied so there appears to be little advantage to females that leave their nest site. Third, my experiment may have altered social interactions among females, as similarly reported in male collared flycatchers (Ficedula albicollis) following the experimental enlargement of their forehead patch size (Qvarnström, 1997). Since nest usurpation by intruding female conspecifics occurs frequently in tree swallows (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985), females that left their nest site after being manipulated were likely unable to defend their nest site from conspecifics; male tree swallows are rarely involved in defence against intruding females and readily breed with the female that takes over the nest site (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). Since female tree swallows defending nest sites often suffer serious injury or death (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985), this would also provide an explanation for why so few females that were usurped were never observed breeding at my study area, although I cannot rule out that they bred in subsequent years in areas outside my study area. Overall, the finding that plumage brightness 51 treatment influenced nest site retention suggests that agonistic interactions among female tree swallows may enforce signal honesty, and the cost of dishonestly signaling high quality may be usurpation. Since the average life expectancy of tree swallows is 2.7 years (Winkler et al., 2011), females unable to retain their nest site may have to forego breeding, thereby imposing fitness costs on unsuccessful females. I predicted that if my results were consistent with the incongruence hypothesis (Rohwer and Rohwer, 1978; Tibbetts and Izzo, 2010), females in both the enhanced and reduced plumage brightness treatments would be less likely to retain their nest site due to increased agonistic interactions from conspecifics (Table 3.1); however, contrary to these predictions, nest site retention did not differ between females in the enhanced and control plumage brightness treatments, and females with reduced plumage brightness tended to be more likely than control females to retain their nest site (Table 3.2). This suggests that females signaling dishonestly high or low quality did not experience increased aggression or attacks from conspecifics as a result of incongruent signals. Although it is possible that a social feedback mechanism altered the behaviour of females so that it was congruent with the quality signalled by their plumage brightness treatment (Vitousek et al., 2014b), I believe this is unlikely to explain my results for tree swallows for two reasons. First, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment were not more likely than controls to retain their nest site, which is inconsistent with previous studies showing that subordinates whose traits were manipulated to signal high status became dominant against rivals following manipulation (Rohwer, 1985); however, I do recognise that females in my study could have been usurped from their nest site before feedback from repeated interactions with conspecifics altered hormone levels and subsequently their behaviour. Second, if the behaviour of females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment did become congruent with that of a low-quality 52 signaller, then fewer females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment should have retained their nest site compared to control females either because they were more likely to be usurped or to abandon their nest if interactions with conspecifics were stressful (Berzins and Dawson 2016); instead, I observed the opposite (Table 3.2). An alternative mechanism that is hypothesized to maintain the honesty of signals is social control (Rohwer, 1977). That fewer female tree swallows in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment retained their nest site compared to females with reduced plumage brightness (Table 3.2) is consistent with the social control hypothesis. If challenged aggressively and frequently by naturally bright females, then females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have been less able to defend their nest site from intruders of greater competitive ability. Since social control can only occur in species that exhibit likeversus-like aggression between high-quality individuals (Senar, 2006), the finding that control females also tended to be less likely to retain their nest site than females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment suggests that conspecifics challenge signal quality by way of nest site intrusions. The results for females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment may also support the social control hypothesis if reduced females were better able to defend and retain their nest site against similarly low- or mid-quality conspecifics with poor or equivalent competitive ability; however, this explanation of my results suggests that dishonestly signaling low quality is advantageous to females because they are more likely to breed. Without a cost imposed on dishonest signallers, such a signaling system would be susceptible to dishonesty (e.g., Owens and Hartley, 1991). Therefore, it is also possible that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment were able to retain their nest site simply because they were involved in fewer agonistic interactions overall with brighter females. For 53 example, less ornamented second-year female tree swallows receive fewer intrusions onto their own territories by older, more ornamented females (Coady and Dawson 2013). Whether or not signals of quality are challenged may depend on the value of the resource and costs of conflict (Maynard Smith and Harper 1988). For example, conspecific paper wasps (Polistes dominulus) challenged food guards when the value of the resource was high (Tibbetts, 2008), but when the value of the resource was low, conspecifics only challenged those food guards signaling low quality and avoided those signaling high quality (Tibbetts and Lindsay, 2008). My results for tree swallows may similarly depend on the value of the resource; conspecifics may challenge females to test signal quality prior to breeding when the value of obtaining a nest site is greater than the cost of fighting. Depreciation in the value of nest sites toward the end of the breeding season may explain why my results differ from a previous study that altered the plumage brightness of female tree swallows after the hatching of eggs; females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment were more likely to abandon their nesting attempt when rearing an experimentally enlarged brood compared to females in enhanced and control plumage brightness treatments (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). These results may be due to enhanced females signaling high quality receiving fewer intrusions or agonistic interactions than females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment (Berzins and Dawson, 2016), as described above in paper wasps when the value of the resource is low (Tibbetts and Lindsay, 2008). Collectively, the results of the two studies suggest that bright plumage displayed by females is tested when the contested resource is valuable (this study), but trusted when the value of the resource is reduced (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). Dale and Slagsvold (1995) reported that the outcome of contests over nest sites in female pied flycatchers was determined by the value of the breeding opportunity to the female and not asymmetries in resource-holding potential; this 54 also has been suggested in other studies examining female contest behaviour (see Elias et al., 2010 and references therein). That nest site retention by female tree swallows was not related to nest-site quality or the original plumage colour of females provides further evidence that signal quality is tested because breeding opportunities to female tree swallows are valuable. Social costs imposed on dishonest signallers may include increased aggression from conspecifics, lower reproductive success, or increased energy expenditure (Kotiaho, 2001). I recognize that by measuring nest site retention I was unable to directly observe any aggressive interactions among females; however, manipulating wild female birds prior to breeding allowed me to examine whether there were social costs for female reproductive success. Indeed, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment that dishonestly signalled high quality suffered social costs since they initiated their clutches later than females in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments (Figure 3.1a). Delayed breeding for females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have been due to defending their nest site from naturally bright females who are likely superior competitors since they pair with bright, high-quality male tree swallows (Bitton et al., 2008). Defence of nest sites against superior competitors may reduce resources available for reproduction and self-maintenance. As such, social costs should be considered in future studies examining trade-offs between ornamentation and fecundity (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995) because, as my results show, social costs of possessing ornamental traits may also lower female reproductive success. Whether such costs translate into lower offspring quality remains to be tested, but correlative studies in tree swallows suggest that females that are more ornamented, and presumably of higher quality (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013), produce offspring of lower quality (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Moreover, interactions with conspecifics may increase social instability resulting in elevated levels of 55 the stress hormone corticosterone (Haller et al., 1999), which may also explain why more ornamented female tree swallows have greater levels of nest parasitism, poorer immune defences, and lower hematocrit levels (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). This is supported by a recent study in pied flycatchers that reported higher levels of blood malondialdehydes, indicative of oxidative damage, in non-ornamented females manipulated to display a forehead patch compared to control females (Moreno et al. 2013). In conclusion, the results of my experiment are consistent with Moreno et al. (2013) in demonstrating that the honesty of signals in female birds may be enforced by social control during the breeding season when the value of breeding opportunities is high. Fewer females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment were able to defend and retain their nest site compared to females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment (Table 3.2), and a similar trend was observed for control females compared to those in the reduced plumage brightness treatment, which supports previous studies demonstrating that individuals of similar signal quality engage in more aggressive interactions (Møller, 1987). Although nest site retention did not differ between the enhanced and control plumage brightness treatments, the delayed breeding date for females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment is consistent with a social cost imposed on females whose elaborate ornamentation is challenged by high-quality females. As such, social costs of displaying ornamented plumage may reduce resources available for reproduction, as hypothesized for production or maintenance costs of ornamental traits (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995). Additional studies that experimentally manipulate the ornamental traits of females prior to breeding would greatly improve our understanding of social interactions among females and the costs of displaying such traits on female reproductive success. 56 Table 3.1. Predictions for whether female tree swallows retain their nest site or are usurped following a plumage brightness manipulation to reduce or enhance plumage brightness compared to controls. 57 Plumage brightness treatment Predictions of nest retention Rationale for nest site retention prediction Reduced Usurp Control Retain Enhanced Usurp Individuals manipulated to signal high quality receive more aggression Reduced Retain Social control hypothesis Individuals manipulated to signal low quality are challenged by lowquality signallers Control Usurp Enhanced Usurp Reduced Usurp Control Retain Enhanced Retain Incongruence hypothesis Individuals manipulated to signal low quality receive more aggression Individuals manipulated to signal high quality are challenged by highquality signallers Social feedback hypothesis Individuals manipulated to signal low quality receive more aggression and adopt the behaviour of a low-quality signaller Individuals manipulated to signal high quality receive less aggression and adopt the behaviour of a high-quality signaller Reference Ferns and Hinsley 2004 Tibbetts and Izzo, 2010 Rohwer 1977 Møller, 1987 Dey et al., 2014 Rohwer, 1985 Table 3.2. The number of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced and enhanced, or remained unchanged (control), prior to breeding in 2010 and 2011, and the number of females in each treatment that retained or left their nest box, and successfully bred following manipulation. Nine females were manipulated in both years and are only represented once in the data set (see text for details). Treatment Manipulated Retained box Left box Reduced Control Enhanced 26 27 27 20 14 11 6 13 16 Found new box 4 5 4 Total number of breeding females after manipulation 24 19 15 58 Figure 3.1. Mean (± SE) clutch initiation date of female tree swallows according to a) whether their plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls), and b) whether they retained or switched nest sites following treatment. Clutch initiation dates were standardized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation 1 for experimental nests separately for each year. Negative values indicate clutches initiated early, while positive values indicate clutches initiated later in the breeding season. Sample sizes are given above error bars. 59 Chapter 4: Does bright plumage enhance extra-pair mating success of female passerines? An experiment with tree swallows 4.1. Abstract Recent empirical evidence suggests that ornamental traits displayed by females may have a signalling function during competitive interactions and/or mate attraction; however, less is known about how such traits influence the social and extra-pair mating success of females. To examine whether the attractiveness of female ornamentation influences the quality of social and extra-pair mates, and the proportion of extra-pair paternity in the broods of females, I experimentally enhanced and reduced the plumage brightness of female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) relative to controls. Contrary to my predictions, the quality of social mate acquired by females did not differ by plumage brightness treatment. Moreover, the rate of extra-pair paternity was similar among females regardless of plumage brightness treatment, and I found no evidence to suggest that this was due to social mates influencing extra-pair mating opportunities of females in response to the their perceived attractiveness. Consistent with previous studies in my population, pairwise comparisons showed that social and extra-pair mates did not differ for any phenotypic traits measured in the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments; however, the extra-pair mates of females with enhanced plumage brightness had longer flight feathers than social mates. This result may be due to social feedback from conspecifics about signal quality influencing female choice or ability to pursue high-quality extra-pair mates. Overall, my study shows that female tree swallows displaying bright plumage have greater extra-pair mating success, and highlights the importance of manipulating the ornamental quality of females when examining extra-pair mating decisions. 60 4.2. Introduction Ornamental traits may evolve by sexual selection if such traits increase the mating success of their bearer and result in differential reproductive success (Darwin, 1871). Elaborately ornamented males may have greater mating success if their trait provides an advantage in direct competition with conspecifics for mates or if females prefer traits that are more attractive (Andersson, 1994). Data from empirical studies have demonstrated that males displaying attractive ornaments have greater success at copulating with females or acquiring mates (Johnsen et al., 1998; Loyau et al., 2007), maintaining paternity within their brood (Estep et al., 2005; Safran et al., 2005), or gaining extra-pair fertilizations (Bitton et al., 2007; Albrecht et al., 2009). In species where both sexes display similar ornamentation, female traits may evolve by sexual or social selection (Clutton-Brock, 2009; Tobias et al., 2012). Elaborate ornaments displayed by females may function as signals of attractiveness that are preferred by males (Amundsen et al., 1997) or as signals of competitive ability or social status during competition with conspecific females (Murphy et al., 2009b). Ornamental traits of females that evolve by competition with conspecifics may also function secondarily in attracting mates (LeBas, 2006). Such traits are known to have dual utility (see review in Berglund et al., 1996), and traits that function in both contexts have been demonstrated in female birds (e.g., Griggio et al., 2010). Although ornamental traits of females have been shown to function during competitive interactions and /or mate attraction, less is known about how such traits influence the social and extra-pair mating success of free-living female animals. Experimentally altering the attractiveness of female ornamental traits has been shown to influence courtship and copulation frequency (Pilastro et al., 2003; Griggio et al., 2005; Torres and Velando, 2005) and sperm allocation by males (Cornwallis and Birkhead, 2007). 61 For example, reducing the breast patch size of female rock sparrows (Petronia petronia) lowered the rate of courtship and sexual chases received from males (Griggio et al., 2005). Females with a reduced ornament also were less likely to pair with a social mate than control females (Griggio et al., 2005); however, it still remains to be determined how altered attractiveness of female ornamentation influences the quality of social mate they acquire. In addition, ornamental traits of females may also influence extra-pair mating success if females seek extra-pair copulations to obtain genetic benefits for their offspring, such as good genes, compatible genes, or ‘sexy sons’ (reviewed in Birkhead and Møller, 1992). Although males are predicted to be less discriminating in their choice of extra-pair than social mates (Trivers, 1972, also see Krebs et al., 2004; Hasegawa et al., 2015 for examples), a previous study in blue-footed boobies (Sula nebouxii) demonstrated that females manipulated to display dull foot colour received less courtship attempts not only from social mates, but potential extrapair mates as well (Torres and Velando, 2005). As such, greater opportunities to engage in extra-pair copulations may arise for females with elaborate ornamentation if they are able to attract extra-pair mates or if social feedback from mates following signal enhancement increases a female’s motivation to pursue extra-pair copulations (Burley, 1988). Additionally, females whose ornaments signal high social status or greater competitive ability may be able to secure extra-pair copulations by invading the territories of other females in search of extra-pair mates. In contrast, females with attractive ornaments may be less likely to engage in extra-pair copulations if they are paired to high-quality mates (e.g., Safran et al., 2005). Indeed, more ornamented female yellow warblers (Setophaga petechial) had fewer extra-pair offspring in their broods compared to less-ornamented females (Grunst and Grunst, 2014). Finally, it is also possible that the ornamentation of females is unrelated 62 to the proportion of extra-pair paternity, as previously demonstrated in western bluebirds (Sialia mexicana; Jacobs et al., 2015). In addition to ornamentation, a female’s success at acquiring extra-pair copulations also may depend on the paternity guards of her social mate; for example, males may mate guard or copulate frequently with their mates to reduce cuckoldry (reviewed in Birkhead and Møller, 1992). Since paternity guards may be costly (Komdeur, 2001) and males may face a trade-off between guarding their mate and gaining extra-pair fertilizations themselves (Chuang-Dobbs et al., 2001), factors such as a female’s opportunity to engage in extra-pair copulations (Wilson and Swaddle, 2013) or the attractiveness of male neighbours (Estep et al., 2005) may influence whether a male guards his mate. In cattle egrets (Bubulcus ibis), males invest more time at the nest and copulate more frequently when paired to females with more ornamented plumage (Krebs et al., 2004). Similarly, a study in humans (Homo sapiens) reported that female attractiveness increases the frequency of within-pair copulations (Kaighobadi and Shackelford, 2008). Male rock sparrows also increase the time spent at the nest when paired with females manipulated to display larger breast patches, which may enable them to gain fertilizations in the second brood produced by the female (Pilastro et al., 2003). Therefore, the attractiveness of female ornaments may also influence the proportion of extra-pair paternity within a brood if males employ strategies to protect their paternity and consequently their success at gaining extra-pair fertilizations, but as far as I am aware, this has never been tested experimentally. In the current study, my aim was to test whether attractive ornamentation influences social and extra-pair mating success and the opportunity for extra-pair mating in female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Females that are in at least their second breeding season (after-second-year; ASY) display dorsal plumage that is similar to males (bright iridescent 63 blue-green), and evidence from previous studies suggests that plumage of ornamented, higher quality females is brighter, with greater ultraviolet (UV) and blue chroma, and reflects light at shorter wavelengths (Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Females and males assortatively mate for plumage brightness (Bitton et al., 2008), and previous studies suggest that plumage brightness is a signal assessed by female conspecifics during agonistic interactions (Berzins and Dawson, 2016; Chapter 3); nevertheless, plumage brightness of females may be a trait that also functions by attracting social or extra-pair mates. Therefore, I experimentally enhanced and reduced plumage brightness prior to breeding of female tree swallows relative to controls (see Berzins and Dawson, 2016) to test whether female attractiveness influences social and extra-pair mate quality, and the proportion of extra-pair paternity in the brood of the female. Although male tree swallows do not guard their social mates (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1984), males that frequently copulate with their social mate sire more within-pair offspring (Crowe et al., 2009). If this behaviour is influenced by female attractiveness, then males socially paired to females with varying plumage brightness may differ in their extra-pair mating success. Therefore, I also tested whether the plumage brightness treatment of a female influenced whether their mates sired extra-pair offspring in the broods of other females, the number of within-pair offspring sired, and the total number of offspring (within- and extra-pair combined) sired by males. I predicted that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment would be perceived as more attractive to males and thus obtain social mates with brighter plumage than females in the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments (Bitton et al., 2008). I also predicted that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment would have fewer extra-pair offspring in their brood if their social mate copulated with them frequently to protect their paternity, but that enhanced females would attract extra-pair mates of higher quality (i.e., larger body size or 64 brighter plumage) than the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments. In contrast, I predicted that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment would have a greater proportion of extra-pair paternity in their brood, engage in extra-pair copulations with males of poorer quality than their social mate, and that their social mate would be more likely to pursue extra-pair copulations than invest in paternity assurance through frequent within-pair copulations. 4.3. Material and methods 4.3.1. Study area and general field methods My study was conducted on tree swallows breeding in nest boxes near Prince George BC, Canada (53ºN, 123ºW) from May to August in 2010 and 2011. The study area consisted of open agricultural areas and small wetlands with patches of coniferous and deciduous trees (see Dawson et al., 2005 for more details). During early May, I began visiting nests daily to document nest building, and captured females in nest boxes once the construction of a nest cup was complete, but before any eggs had been laid. Each female was banded and weighed using a spring balance (nearest 0.25 g). I measured the lengths of the ninth primary flight feather, wing chord and outer rectrix feather using a ruler (nearest 0.5 mm) and combined head and bill (hereafter, head-bill) with digital calipers (nearest 0.1 mm). After determining the age of females using dorsal plumage colour (Hussell, 1983), I sequentially allocated each ASY female to one of three plumage brightness treatments (enhanced, reduced, control), with the treatment order determined randomly. Details of the plumage brightness manipulation are described in Berzins and Dawson (2016). Briefly, I evenly applied to the dorsal feathers a non-toxic permanent blue marker to enhance or silicon paste to reduce the plumage brightness of females; control females were treated in the same manner, but the 65 marker used contained water. The markers and silicone paste I used in my study have been used previously in other studies to manipulate plumage colour (e.g., Safran et al., 2005; Johnsen et al., 2005), and I was confident that my treatment produced the desired effect throughout the experimental period because the effects of these markers and paste have been detected for on average 15 days after their application (Johnsen et al., 2005). After the plumage brightness treatment, the brightness of female plumage fell within the natural range for female tree swallows at my study area (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). Age, body mass, and plumage characteristics of females did not differ by plumage brightness treatment prior to manipulation (all P values > 0.29). Plumage brightness was enhanced for 30 females and reduced for 31 females, whereas the plumage brightness of 28 females remained the same. Following the manipulation of female plumage brightness, I monitored all nests at my study area and recorded the dates of clutch initiation and hatching (where January 1 = 1). The date the first egg in each nest hatched was designated as day 0 of the nestling period. I captured adults when they were feeding nestlings, and measured each bird as described above. The band number of previously manipulated females was recorded, and males were banded (if not previously banded) with an individually numbered aluminum leg band. I also collected five rump feathers from each male, which were stored in opaque envelopes at room temperature until spectral analysis (details below). For the paternity analysis, a small blood sample (20 µL) was collected from the brachial vein of each adult and twelve-day old nestling using non-heparinized capillary tubes. Blood samples were transferred into microfuge tubes containing Queen’s lysis buffer (Seutin et al., 1991) and were stored at -20 oC until DNA extraction. Eggs that failed to hatch and nestlings that died prior to blood sampling were collected and stored at -20 oC. 66 4.3.2. Spectral analysis Plumage characteristics of tree swallow feathers were quantified by taping four rump feathers to a piece of cardboard so that they mimicked the natural arrangement of rump feathers on a bird (Bitton et al., 2007), and using a JAZ-PX spectrometer with a xenon light source (OceanOptics, Dunedin, FL, USA) and a bifurcated probe enclosed in a black plastic holder to quantify reflectance spectra. Three measurements were taken for each sample and spectral data were recorded using SpectraSuite software (OceanOptics, Dunedin, FL, USA). The R package pavo (Maia et al., 2013) was used to smooth spectral curves and quantify hue, ultraviolet (UV) chroma, blue chroma and average brightness; these plumage characteristics have previously been shown to reflect aspects of male quality in tree swallows (Bitton et al., 2007; Bitton and Dawson, 2008, Bitton et al., 2008). The three measures for each of the plumage characteristics were averaged, and then I used a principal components analysis (PCA) to reduce UV chroma, blue chroma, and hue into individual colour scores that were used in analyses (Montgomerie, 2006). Average brightness was not included as a variable in the PCA because it was not correlated with UV chroma, blue chroma, or hue (all P values > 0.12). In this PCA, I included all male tree swallows over a four-year period (2010-2013) from which I had collected feather samples. The first principal component (PC1) explained 64.9 % of the total variation and the factor loadings (hue: -0.92; UV chroma: 0.97; blue chroma: 0.16) suggest males with larger positive PC1 scores have plumage with greater UV chroma that reflects light at shorter wavelengths, i.e., were bluer. The second principal component (PC2) explained 34.2 % of the total variation and was heavily weighted by blue chroma (-0.06, -0.22, 0.99, respectively), so males with larger positive scores have plumage with greater blue chroma. Previous studies have shown that males with brighter plumage sire a greater number of extra-pair offspring than duller males (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham 67 and Dunn, 2016). Older males, based on recapture status (see below), have plumage that is brighter and bluer than younger males, and among duller males those that are bluer are more likely to return to my study area to breed (Bitton and Dawson, 2008). These relationships suggest that males displaying plumage that is brighter, with greater UV and blue chroma, and that reflects light maximally at shorter wavelengths (larger positive PC1 and PC2 scores) are ‘more ornamented’ because they have greater reproductive success and/or apparent return rates. 4.3.3. Paternity analysis The paternity analysis for this study was conducted as part of a larger three-year study (2010-2012). DNA from blood and tissue samples was extracted using DNeasy Blood & Tissue kits (Qiagen #69506), and amplified at either five or six microsatellite loci developed for Tachycineta swallows (Tle19, TaBi34, TaBi6, Tal8, TaBi10 and TaBi 8; Makarewich et al., 2009). I performed duplex polymerase chain reaction (PCR) with two primer pairs, TaBi6/Tle19 and Tal8/TaBi34, and PCR for TaBi10 and TaBi8 as single reactions. Amplification of DNA extracted from blood samples was performed in 10 L final volume containing 1x PCR buffer, 0.2 mM of each dNTP (Invitrogen #10297-018), 1.75– 2.25 mM MgCl2, 0.4 g/L bovine serum albumin (BSA; New England Biolabs #B90015), 0.08–0.2 M of forward-labelled and reverse primers, 0.05 U/L Taq polymerase (Invitrogen #10342-020), and 20–50 ng of genomic DNA. PCR was performed in a MJ Research Peltier Thermal Cycler under the following PCR cycling protocol: an initial denaturing step at 95 °C for 4 min, followed by 35 cycles of 95 °C for 50 sec, 56–60 °C for 60 sec (56 °C for the duplex reactions and 60 °C for TaBi10 andTaBi8), and 65-72 °C for 90 sec (65 °C for the duplex reactions and 72 °C for TaBi10 andTaBi8). A final extension cycle 68 at 65–72 °C for 5 min (65 °C for duplex reactions and 72 °C for TaBi10 andTaBi8) completed the PCR. Amplification of DNA extracted from tissue was performed using a Qiagen Multiplex PCR kit (Qiagen #206143) following manufacturer’s instructions, except I used a final volume of 10 L and 3–5 L of Multiplex PCR Master Mix. Concentrations of primers and annealing temperatures remained the same as described above. Products from PCR were run on an ABI 3130xl automated sequencer (Applied Biosystems), and data were analyzed blind to sample identity using Peak ScannerTM Software v1.0 (Applied Biosystems). Originally, all individuals from 2010 and 2011 were genotyped at five loci: Tle19, TaBi34, TaBi6, Tal8, and TaBi10; however, due to a high frequency of null alleles for Tal8 in my study population, I also genotyped all males and extra-pair offspring that were homozygous at Tal8 plus the genetic mother of those offspring at a sixth locus (TaBi8) so that 5 loci were used to identify genetic sires (see below). Starting in 2012, I genotyped individuals at all six loci. I calculated allele frequencies and exclusion probabilities for all microsatellite loci in my study population using CERVUS 3.0.7 (Kalinowski et al., 2007; see Table 4.1). TaBi8 significantly deviated from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (P = 0.002; Table 4.1), but this locus was never used as a single criterion to determine parentage. Offspring were classified as within-pair if they matched the resident male at five loci and extra-pair offspring if they mismatched the resident male at a minimum of one locus, but to account for the presence of null alleles a single mismatch was not included if either the offspring or resident were homozygous at the mismatching loci (O’Brien and Dawson, 2007). For this experiment, no offspring mismatched the resident male at only a single locus; all offspring identified as extra-pair mismatched the resident male at two or more loci. Among nests used in this study, no germinal disk or embryonic 69 tissue was observed in ten eggs (from three enhanced, three reduced, and two control nests), and these eggs were considered as unfertilized for the paternity analysis. For this study, I was interested in identifying the genetic sires of extra-pair offspring in experimental broods and identifying extra-pair offspring sired by a female’s social mate. To assign parentage of extra-pair offspring to a genetic sire I compared the alleles of each extra-pair offspring to the alleles of all males sampled at my study separately for 2010 and 2011, and males that matched an extra-pair offspring at all five loci were assigned as the extra-pair sire (only one sire was ever identified for each extra-pair offspring). Based on this criterion, I assigned parentage to 155 of the 255 extra-pair offspring identified at my study area in 2010 and 2011, and for those offspring I calculated the probability of chance inclusion following O’Brien and Dawson (2007). Overall, the probability that a randomly chosen male would match an extra-pair offspring at all five loci ranged from 3.0 x 10-7 to 0.0091 (mean ± SE: 0.00071 ± 0.00012). There were 7 additional cases where an extra-pair offspring and putative genetic sire matched at four loci, but mismatched at a fifth locus because either the offspring or putative genetic sire was homozygous. For two of these cases, the offspring shared alleles with a maternal half-sibling that matched the same genetic sire at all five loci, and in two other cases, the genetically related offspring of the putative genetic sire also mismatched at the same locus. Nevertheless, even at only four loci the chance that a random male would match these extra-pair offspring at all four loci was low (0.00094 ± 0.00032; range 0.00025 to 0.0025). 4.3.4. Statistical analysis I tested whether the quality of a female’s social mate differed by plumage brightness treatment using random intercept linear mixed models (LMM; lme4 package, Bates et al., 70 2015). In these models, I included females that were manipulated in both years, but only if they paired with a different male in each year (N = 5); one female paired with the same male in both years so only data from the first year of the experiment were included in the models. Identity of females and males were both included as random factors to account for multiple observations of the same individuals. Each phenotypic trait of males (body condition, lengths of wing, ninth primary feather, outer rectrix, and head-bill, UV chroma-hue (PC1), blue chroma (PC2), and brightness) was a dependent variable, and plumage brightness treatment was included as a fixed factor. Body condition was estimated by calculating the residuals from a regression between body mass and length of the head-bill for all males captured at the study area from 2010 to 2013 (F1, 217 = 23.81, P < 0.00001; Brown 1996). The age of nestlings at capture was included as a covariate for models examining body condition to account for declines in body mass during the nestling provisioning stage (Boyle et al., 2012). Hatching date was not included as a covariate in models because timing of breeding is confounded with individual quality (e.g., Dawson, 2008). Since phenotypic traits of male tree swallows, such as the length of ninth primary and plumage brightness, increase with age (Bitton and Dawson, 2008; O’Brien, 2006), I also included the capture status (previously banded or unbanded) of males as a proxy for age (see Bitton and Dawson, 2008; Bitton et al., 2007). Previously banded males that return to breed are presumed to be older than unbanded males breeding at my study area for the first time because male tree swallows rarely change breeding sites between years (i.e., less than 4 %, see Winkler et al., 2004). Males banded as nestlings in the previous breeding season were grouped with unbanded males. Year was not included in models as it was confounded with capture status. Females that switched nest boxes after being manipulated bred later and in poorer quality nest boxes than they had occupied prior to switching (Chapter 3), so I also included female status (retain or switch 71 nest boxes) as a categorical variable. P values for these LMM were calculated using the R package lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2016), and I removed predictor variables that did not approach significance (P ≥ 0.10) in a backwards, stepwise fashion, but always retained plumage brightness treatment in models. Samples sizes differ among phenotypic traits because in some cases, feathers were damaged or broken and so were not measured. To determine whether variation in plumage brightness of females influenced their opportunity for extra-pair mating, I first tested whether the presence of extra-pair offspring (yes or no) within a female’s brood was related to plumage brightness treatment using a likelihood ratio test. Females that were manipulated and bred in both years were randomly assigned to either 2010 or 2011 for this analysis since I had multiple observations for some females (N = 4). I also tested whether the number of extra-pair young within nests differed by the plumage brightness treatment of the female using a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM; lme4 package, Bates et al., 2015) fitted with binomial errors and logit link function for proportion data (Crawley, 2013). The number of extra-pair offspring was the dependent variable, the number of offspring in the brood was the binomial denominator, and plumage brightness treatment and year were included as fixed factors. The number of days a female was manipulated (i.e., clutch initiation date – manipulation date; range 1 to 21, mean = 9.7 ± 0.8 SE) prior to initiating a clutch was included as a covariate. To account for the multiple observations of females represented in the data set in both years I included female identity as a random factor. The model was tested for overdispersion using the RVAideMemoie package (Hervé, 2016), and I included an observation-level random factor in the model to account for overdispersion (Browne et al., 2005). The significance of fixed effects and covariates was examined by assessing the change in deviance between models (Crawley, 2013), and I report Wald χ2 tests calculated using the Car package (Fox and Weisberg, 2011). For both analyses, 72 I only included broods where the paternity data were complete (i.e., the paternity status was known for every egg laid by the female). I tested whether the plumage brightness treatment of females influenced the quality of their extra-pair mates by comparing phenotypic traits between social and extra-pair mates within each brood using a series of LMM. Male paternity status (social or extra-pair mate) was defined as the repeated effect and pair identity was the subject grouping variable. Each phenotypic trait (condition, lengths of wing, ninth primary, outer rectrix, and head-bill, average brightness, UV chroma-hue (PC1), and blue chroma (PC2)) was the dependent variable in individual models, and I included plumage brightness treatment and male paternity status as fixed effects, and the interaction between the two factors. Since my objective was to test whether the plumage brightness treatment of females influenced the quality of their social mates, models always retained plumage brightness treatment, male paternity status, and their interaction. Some males were missing measurements because they had damaged or broken feathers, so in these analyses I included only those males where the majority (or all) phenotypic traits were measured so that I could compare as many traits as possible between a female’s social and extra-pair mate. To determine whether altering the perceived attractiveness of females influenced male mating strategies, I tested whether social males sired extra-pair offspring (yes or no) in relation to the plumage brightness treatment of the female using a likelihood ratio test. I also tested whether the number of within-pair and total number of offspring sired by social mates differed according the plumage brightness treatment of the female using a generalized linear model (GLM) fitted with a quasi-Poisson error structure and log link function for overdispersed count data (Crawley, 2013). Since the brightness of male plumage and capture status may influence male mating strategies (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 73 2016), I included both plumage brightness and capture status of males in models. As described above, the significance of fixed factors and covariates was examined by assessing the change in deviance between models. For females that were captured and manipulated in both years (N = 6), I randomly selected only data for one of their social mates to be included in these analyses. Results were considered significant at P ≤ 0.05, and means are presented + 1 standard error. When appropriate, parameter estimates (B) and effect sizes are reported. Effects sizes were calculated as the correlation coefficient (r) following Field et al. (2012). All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v20 statistical software (IBM Corp., 2011) and R v3.1.1 (R Development Core Team, 2015). 4.4. Results The quality of social mate paired to females did not differ by plumage brightness treatment (all P values > 0.12; Table 4.2), and females that switched nest sites after being manipulated did not socially pair with males of lower phenotypic quality than females that retained their nest sites (all P values > 0.25). Although body condition of males was similar among plumage brightness treatments (Table 4.2), it was negatively related to the age of nestlings at capture (B = -0.07 ± 0.03, F1, 19.58 = 5.13, P = 0.04). No effect of capture status on male phenotypic traits was detected (all P values > 0.11) and so this variable was removed from models. Plumage brightness treatment of the female had no effect on whether a nest contained extra-pair offspring or the proportion of extra-pair offspring in a brood (Table 4.3). Year and the number of days females were manipulated prior to initiating a clutch were unrelated to levels of extra-pair paternity (P values > 0.15). The plumage brightness treatment of the 74 female also did not influence whether or not her social mate sired offspring in other broods (Table 4.3), or the number of within-pair offspring or the total number of offspring her social mate sired (Table 4.3). Capture status and plumage brightness of males were unrelated to the number of within-pair and total number of offspring sired (all P values > 0.13) and were removed from models. Comparisons of social and extra-pair mates for length of ninth primary flight feathers suggested that the effect of female plumage brightness treatment varied with male paternity status (Table 4.4), so I analyzed the data separately by plumage brightness treatment. In the enhanced plumage brightness treatment, extra-pair mates tended to have longer ninth primary feathers than social mates (B = 5.50 ± 1.61, F1, 2.00 = 11.71, P = 0.076, r = 0.92, Figure 4.1c); no differences were detected between extra-pair and social mates in either the control (B = -2.50 ± 2.22, F1, 3.00 = 1.27, P = 0.34, r = -0.54) or reduced plumage brightness treatments (B = -1.20 ± 1.83, F1, 4.00 = 0.43, P = 0.54, r = -0.31). The analysis of length of outer rectrix also showed an interaction between plumage brightness treatment and male paternity status (Table 4.3). Analyzing the data separately by plumage brightness treatment showed that extra-pair mates had longer outer rectrices than social mates in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment (B = 2.33 ± 0.82, F1, 9.36 = 8.03, P = 0.02, r = 0.68, Figure 4.1d), but did not differ in the control (B = -0.71 ± 1.13, F1, 10.03 = 0.40, P = 0.54, r = -0.19) and reduced plumage brightness treatments (B = -1.25 ± 0.78, F1, 13.64 = 2.06, P = 0.17, r = -0.36). Body condition did not differ between social and extra-pair mates or plumage brightness treatment, but was negatively related to the age of nestlings at capture (B = -0.081 ± 0.04; Table 4.4). Although extra-pair mates overall had plumage with greater blue chroma than social mates (B = 0.81 ± 0.32; Figure 4.3), the magnitude of this difference was unrelated to the plumage brightness treatment of the female (Table 4.4). Length of the wing and head-bill, plumage 75 brightness, and UV chroma-hue (PC1) did not differ between social and extra-pair mates or the plumage brightness treatment of the female (Table 4.4). 4.5. Discussion In the current study my aim was to alter the brightness of female plumage to examine whether female attractiveness influenced social and extra-pair mating success and a female’s opportunity for extra-pair mating. Contrary to my predictions, the quality of social mate paired to females was similar regardless of plumage brightness treatment (Table 4.2). Moreover, a similar proportion of extra-pair offspring in the broods of females (Table 4.3) indicates that the plumage brightness treatment did not influence a female’s success at acquiring extra-pair copulations. This is also supported by the percentage of broods containing extra-pair paternity (see Table 4.3, overall 84 %), which is consistent with the rate of extra-pair paternity reported previously at my study area and others (see O’Brien and Dawson, 2007 and references therein; Chapter 6). Despite signaling lower quality plumage, females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment did not mate with extra-pair males of lower phenotypic quality compared to their social mate (Figures 4.1, 4.2). In contrast, extrapair mates of females in the enhanced plumage treatment had longer ninth primary feathers and outer rectrices than their social males in pairwise comparisons (Figures 4.1c, d). If such traits are indicative of quality or age (O’Brien, 2006), then my results may suggest that female tree swallows displaying bright plumage have greater extra-pair mating success because they secure extra-pair copulations from high-quality extra-pair mates. This finding is in line with previous studies in male tree swallows showing that males with bright plumage have greater extra-pair mating success because they sire a greater number of extra-pair offspring (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016). 76 The attractiveness of female ornamental traits may influence female mating success if such traits enable females to be approached or courted more frequently by males (Pilastro et al., 2003; Griggio et al., 2005; Torres and Velando, 2005), pair successfully (Griggio et al., 2005), or acquire high-quality mates. For example, female rock sparrows manipulated to display a breast patch reduced in size were less likely to pair with a social mate (Griggio et al., 2005), but the quality of social mate acquired by the female was unknown. Since assortative mating for breast patch size was observed in another population of rock sparrows, Griggio et al. (2005) hypothesized that females with reduced breast patches may have mated with low-quality males. Tree swallows also have been shown to assortatively mate for plumage brightness (Bitton et al., 2008), but my study provided no evidence of females displaying enhanced or reduced plumage brightness socially pairing with brighter or duller mates compared to control females (Table 4.2). In fact, there were no phenotypic traits of social mates that differed in relation to the plumage brightness treatment of the female (Table 4.2). Since female plumage brightness was manipulated after pair formation, I may only have detected an effect of female treatment on quality of social mates if males divorced females after the manipulation to pair with a female they perceived to be of higher quality (review in Choudhury, 1995) and manipulated females acquired new social mates. Given that divorce can be costly and unpaired females of high quality may be limited (Choudhury, 1995), males may have pursued extra-pair copulations with females of higher quality than their social mate; however, I also detected no evidence that the plumage brightness treatment of the female influenced whether her social mate sired offspring in the broods of other females (Table 4.3). As such, the most likely explanation for my results is that plumage brightness is not a signal of attractiveness that influences the behaviour of male tree swallows. This is consistent with a previous study that reported no evidence of differential allocation by male 77 tree swallows in relation to plumage brightness treatment of females they were paired with (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). Instead, bright plumage of female tree swallows appears to be a signal assessed by female conspecifics (Berzins and Dawson, 2016, Chapter 3) and likely functions predominately in a competitive context, as demonstrated previously in female American goldfinches (Spinus tristis; Murphy et al., 2009b). That the proportion of extra-pair offspring in the broods of females was similar among plumage brightness treatments (Table 4.3) suggests that in tree swallows attractive ornamentation does not increase a female’s opportunity to engage in extra-pair copulations. My results are consistent with a previous study in western bluebirds that reported no relationship between female coloration and the rate of extra-pair paternity within broods (Jacobs et al., 2015). In contrast, female yellow warblers displaying plumage with greater melanin coverage, but lower carotenoid pigmentation had a higher proportion of extra-pair offspring in their broods compared to females with plumage that had both high melanin coverage and carotenoid pigmentation (Grunst and Grunst, 2014). Grunst and Grunst (2014) hypothesized that females whose plumage had low carotenoid pigmentation may have produced broods with a higher proportion of extra-pair offspring if they paired with poorer quality social mates or were mate-guarded less intensively because of their less ornamented dull carotenoid-based plumage and had a greater propensity to seek or engage in extra-pair copulations due to their melanin-based plumage. Such a relationship between extra-pair paternity and melanin-based plumage may exist as a result of a correlated expression with behavioural traits, such as aggression and sexual behaviour (Ducrest et al., 2008). Although there was no evidence that the quality or mating strategy of social mates influenced extrapair paternity in the broods of females whose plumage brightness was manipulated in my study (Tables 4.2, 4.3), a study in tree swallows that treated females with 1,4,6-androstatrien78 3, 17-dione (ATD) and flutamide (F) in combination (ATD+F) to reduce aggressive and sexual behaviour demonstrated that ATD+F females had fewer extra-pair offspring in their broods than control females (Chapter 6). Therefore, a similar correlated expression between female behavioural traits and extra-pair mating behaviour (Forstmeier et al., 2014) may exist in tree swallows and would suggest female behaviour is a more important determinant of whether females engage in extra-pair copulations than ornamentation. This is in line with a previous study in female Lake Eyre dragon lizards (Ctenophorus maculosus) demonstrating that while males tended to increase their sexual interest toward females manipulated to be more ornamented, ultimately the reproductive state of the female influenced how often males copulated with females; males copulated more frequently with receptive than non-receptive or gravid females (Stuart-Fox and Goode, 2014). Despite identifying overall differences in plumage brightness, and lengths of ninth primaries and outer rectrices between males that sired extra-pair offspring and those males that only sired within-pair offspring within my population (O’Brien, 2006; Bitton et al., 2007), no phenotypic traits have been shown to differ in pairwise comparisons between social and extra-pair mates at my study area (O’Brien, 2006; Bitton et al., 2007; but see Whittingham and Dunn, 2014 for differences in other populations). Consistently, my results for the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments showed no differences in phenotypic traits between social and extra-pair mates of females in pairwise comparisons (Figures 4.1, 4.2), but I did find that females manipulated to display enhanced plumage brightness mated with extra-pair males that tended to have longer flight feathers than the social mate they cuckolded (Figure 4.1c, d). Since males that are experienced breeders, and presumably older, have longer ninth primary and outer rectrix feathers (O’Brien, 2006), this may suggest that extra-pair mates of females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment 79 were older, and perhaps of higher quality, than their social mates. Unfortunately, with my limited sample size I was unable to test whether extra-pair mates were in fact older than the social mate they cuckolded. Nevertheless, male tree swallows with longer flight feathers may be higher quality if flight performance is influenced by feather length (Swaddle et al., 1996). If females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment mated with extra-pair mates that were of higher quality than their social mate, there are two possible mechanisms to explain this result. First, previous studies in fowl (Gallus gallus) have shown that highquality males prefer more ornamented females and when copulating with such females produce ejaculates containing not only more sperm, but also seminal fluid that increases the swimming speed of the sperm (Cornwallis and Birkhead, 2007; Cornwallis and O’Connor, 2009). As such, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment in my study may have received higher quality ejaculates from extra-pair mates that outcompeted the sperm of lowquality males; however, since bright plumage of female tree swallows does not appear to be a signal of attractiveness that influences the behaviour of males (Berzins and Dawson, 2016; this study), it is more likely that males mate with any potential extra-pair females if the opportunity arises, as demonstrated in Japanese barn swallows (Hirundo rustica gutturalis; Hasegawa et al., 2015) and other species (Krebs et al., 2004). Second, social feedback from conspecific females about the ornament quality after the manipulation (reviewed in Vitousek et al., 2014b) may have altered female choice of extra-pair mates. Several studies have demonstrated condition-dependent female mate choice (reviewed in Cotton et al., 2006). For example, female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) reared in experimentally reduced broods so that they perceived themselves as high-quality preferred the song of high-quality males (Holveck and Riebel, 2009). Similarly, female tree swallows in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have perceived themselves as high quality after defending and 80 retaining their nest site from conspecific intruders displaying high-quality signals (Chapter 3). Moreover, since females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment had previously defeated conspecifics in agonistic interactions, they may have pursued extra-pair copulations from high-quality males on the territories of other females because experiencing ‘winning’ may influence an individual’s perceived competitive ability and aggressive behaviour, and increase the likelihood of defeating conspecifics in future challenges (i.e., the winner effect; reviewed in Hsu et al., 2006). Although I have no hormone data, this behaviour may be facilitated by changes in physiology, such as androgen levels (Oliveira et al., 2009). Therefore, further studies that employ ornament and/or androgen manipulations combined with observations of extra-pair copulations may provide novel insights into the proximate mechanisms mediating female mating behaviour. Overall, mating strategies of males were not related to the plumage brightness treatment of their social mate (Table 4.3). Although I genotyped almost all offspring produced in each year of study, I recognize that males may have sired extra-pair offspring at feeding or roost sites away from my study area that were not included in my analyses. Nevertheless, the mating strategies of male tree swallows are likely better predicted by the attributes of males themselves as opposed to the ornamentation of their social mate. For example, male tree swallows with brighter ventral plumage sire a greater number of withinpair offspring (Whittingham and Dunn, 2016), while males with brighter dorsal plumage sire a greater number of extra-pair offspring (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016). It is unclear why the results of my study did not similarly show that extra-pair mates had brighter dorsal plumage than the social mate they cuckolded (Table 4.4), as previously reported (Whittingham and Dunn, 2016), but extra-pair mates did have plumage with greater blue chroma than social mates (Table 4.4, Figure 4.3). The difference in results may be due 81 to altering perceived quality of females and social interactions within the study area (see Berzins and Dawson, 2016), which may have altered female choice of extra-pair mates. In conclusion, the finding that quality of social mates and male mating strategies did not differ in relation to the plumage brightness treatment of the female is consistent with Berzins and Dawson (2016) and suggests that male mating and parental care decisions are not influenced by elaborate ornamentation of female tree swallows. As such, positive assortative mating in this species for plumage brightness (see Bitton et al., 2008) is likely due to competition among females to secure a male with a nest site, rather than male mate choice of bright females. Nevertheless, the results of my experiment suggest that bright plumage may increase a female’s extra-pair mating success by enabling females to engage in extrapair copulations with high-quality males, and highlights the need for future studies to manipulate the signal quality of females when examining extra-pair mating decisions. 82 Table 4.1. Variability of alleles for six microsatellite loci used to assign paternity in a population of tree swallows from 2010–2012. Locus n k Pei hobs hexp F(Null) HW Tle19 295 11 0.67 0.85 0.83 -0.0113 NS TaBi34 295 17 0.69 0.85 0.84 -0.0117 NS TaBi6 295 26 0.72 0.80 0.85 0.0287 NS Tal8 292 25 0.85 0.77 0.93 0.0899 NS TaBi10 295 17 0.76 0.89 0.88 -0.0031 NS TaBi8 232 11 0.66 0.77 0.82 0.0300 * n is the number of unrelated adults genotyped at each locus, k is the number of alleles, Pei is the probability of exclusion with one parent (female) known, hobs is the observed heterozygosity, hexp is the expected heterozygosity, F(Null) is the frequency of null alleles for each locus, and HW indicates whether each locus significantly deviates from HardyWeinberg equilibrium, denoted as *. The combined probability of parental exclusion for all six loci was 0.9997. 83 Table 4.2. Results of random intercept linear mixed models comparing phenotypic traits of social males paired to female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. Presented are the mean (± SE) for each phenotypic trait. Sample sizes are indicated by parentheses, and vary because individuals with feathers that were damaged or broken were not measured. See methods for details of calculating condition and plumage colour metrics. Plumage brightness treatment Reduced Control Enhanced 1 Condition -0.10 ± 0.21 (23) -0.23 ± 0.24 (18) -0.40 ± 0.28 (15) Wing (mm) 120.53 ± 0.69 (17) 121.58 ± 0.82 (12) 121.16 ± 0.86 (11) Ninth primary (mm) 95.39 ± 0.61 (17) 95.89 ± 0.72 (12) 94.94 ± 0.80 (11) Outer rectrix (mm) 57.49 ± 0.38 (19) 57.19 ± 0.42 (15) 56.71 ± 0.60 (9) Head-bill (mm) 28.82 ± 0.09 (23) 28.54 ± 0.10 (17) 28.70 ± 0.12 (15) Average brightness 15.16 ± 0.47 (23) 15.92 ± 0.54 (17) 15.85 ± 0.60 (15) 2 UV chroma-hue (PC1) 0.10 ± 0.22 (23) -0.12 ± 0.25 (17) 0.64 ± 0.28 (15) 3 Blue chroma (PC2) -0.06 ± 0.19 (23) -0.32 ± 0.23 (17) -0.05 ± 0.24 (15) 1 Larger, positive values indicate males in better condition. 2 Larger, positive PC1 values indicate males with greater UV chroma and bluer hue plumage. 3 Larger, positive PC2 values indicate males with greater plumage blue chroma. Trait 84 F df P 0.38 0.50 0.40 0.68 2.05 0.72 2.16 0.44 2, 48.59 2, 35.32 2, 36.57 2, 34.34 2, 51.42 2, 51.68 2, 51.74 2, 46.13 0.69 0.61 0.67 0.52 0.14 0.49 0.12 0.64 Table 4.3. Results of likelihood ratio tests, and general or generalized linear mixed models examining whether the plumage brightness treatment (experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged) of female tree swallows influenced female extra-pair mating success (i.e., presence and proportion of extra-pair offspring) or male mating strategies (i.e., whether males gained extra-pair fertilizations, maintained within-pair paternity, and their total number of offspring sired). Presented are raw proportion means (± SE), and sample sizes are indicated by parentheses. Plumage brightness treatment Reduced Control Enhanced Statistic P 94 % (16) 80 % (14) 79 % (10) G2 = 1.78 0.41 Proportion of EPO1 Percent of social mates that sired EPO1 Number WPO2 sired 0.42 ± 0.06 (17) 0.50 ± 0.09 (14) 0.37 ± 0.08 (13) χ22 = 0.80 0.67 50 % (22) 47 % (17) 25 % (12) G2 = 2.24 0.33 3.32 ± 0.24 (22) 3.12 ± 0.54 (17) 3.17 ±0.53 (12) χ22 = 0.14 0.93 Total # offspring sired 4.95 ± 0.55 (22) 4.76 ± 0.65 (17) 3.83 ± 0.73 (12) χ22 = 1.57 0.46 Variable Percent of nests containing extrapair paternity 85 1 2 EPO refers to extra-pair offspring. WPO refers to within-pair offspring. Table 4.4. Results of linear mixed models comparing phenotypic traits between the social and extra-pair mate of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. See methods for details of calculating condition and plumage colour metrics. Phenotypic trait F df P Body condition Plumage brightness treatment Nestling age Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 0.88 4.58 0.03 0.69 2, 32.56 1, 27.50 1, 32.82 2, 32.61 0.42 0.04 0.86 0.51 Wing (mm) Plumage brightness treatment Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 0.17 0.24 1.17 2, 17.52 1, 17.52 2, 17.52 0.85 0.63 0.33 Ninth primary (mm) Plumage brightness treatment Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 1.15 0.26 3.92 2, 9.00 1, 9.00 2, 9.00 0.36 0.62 0.06 Outer rectrix (mm) Plumage brightness treatment Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 0.05 0.09 3.85 2, 34.28 1, 34.28 2, 34.28 0.95 0.76 0.03 Average brightness Plumage brightness treatment Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 1.11 0.28 0.35 2, 33.41 1, 33.41 2, 33.41 0.34 0.60 0.71 UV chroma-hue (PC1) Plumage brightness treatment Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 2.11 1.79 0.75 2, 18.00 1, 18.00 2, 18.00 0.15 0.20 0.49 Blue chroma (PC2) Plumage brightness treatment Paternity status Plumage brightness x paternity status 2.12 7.17 1.99 2, 18.00 1, 18.00 2, 18.00 0.15 0.02 0.17 86 87 Figure 4.1. Mean (± SE) difference in a) body condition, b) wing length, c) ninth primary length, and d) outer rectrix length between social and extra-pair mates of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. See methods for details of calculating condition. Sample sizes indicate the number of pairs in each treatment and are given above error bars. 88 Figure 4.2. Mean (± SE) difference in a) head-bill length, b) average brightness, c) UV chroma-hue (PC1), and d) blue chroma (PC2) between social and extra-pair mates of female tree swallows whose plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. See methods for details of calculating plumage colour metrics. Sample sizes indicate the number of pairs in each treatment and are given above error bars. Figure 4.3. Mean (± SE) blue chroma (PC2) of social and extra-pair mates of female tree swallows. See methods for details of calculating blue chroma (PC2). Sample sizes indicate the number of males and are given above error bars. 89 Chapter 5: Is ornamentation of female passerines related to offspring quality? An experimental alteration of plumage brightness in female tree swallows 5.1. Abstract It is widely recognized that females in a variety of taxa display ‘male-like’ traits, such as elaborate ornamentation, and determining whether such traits evolve by selection or genetic correlation remains a challenge. In mutually ornamented species, females displaying elaborate traits are sometimes found to produce low-quality offspring and this is viewed as support for the genetic correlation hypothesis because males would not benefit from preferring the most ornamented females. Female traits, however, may instead function in competition with conspecifics and while females that are the most elaborately ornamented may be superior competitors, the costs associated with the possession of such traits may influence offspring quality. In tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor), females in their second breeding season and older display bright blue-green iridescent plumage that is similar to males, and previous studies have shown that more ornamented females produce low-quality nestlings. In the current study, I enhanced and reduced the plumage brightness of females relative to controls to test whether the social costs of displaying ornamental plumage influence nestling quality. Although my results showed no difference in nestling quality between the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments, nestlings reared by females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment were structurally smaller than nestlings in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments. This result is consistent with my predictions and previous studies, and suggests that while bright plumage of female tree swallows may evolve by sexual selection, the social costs associated with possessing 90 elaborate ornamentation also may influence nestling quality and should be considered in future studies. 5.2. Introduction Female animals from a wide variety of taxa display ‘male-like’ traits, such as elaborate or brightly coloured plumage, and despite increased recent empirical investigation, explaining the function and evolution of such traits has remained a challenge (e.g., Tarvin and Murphy, 2012; Tobias et al., 2012; Nordeide et al., 2013). In species where both sexes display elaborate ornamentation, such traits may evolve by mutual sexual selection (Kraaijeveld et al., 2007) if they provide an advantage to females in competition for access to mates (Johnson, 1988), or are attractive to or preferred by males (e.g., Torres and Velando, 2005; Griggio et al., 2009). Mutual mate choice may evolve in species where both sexes contribute to parental care and males may therefore benefit from choosing a mate whose ornamentation signals high quality (reviewed in Kraaijeveld et al., 2007). Females whose ornamental traits signal quality should make greater investments in reproduction (Linville et al., 1998; García-Navas et al., 2012) and/or produce offspring of higher quality (i.e., with better immune defences, larger size, and/or better performance, Hidalgo-Garcia, 2006; Kekäläinen et al., 2010; Remeš and Matysioková, 2013). In contrast, when elaborate traits displayed by females appear to not be preferred by males or are not related to the quality of offspring (e.g., Wolf et al., 2004; reviewed in Nordeide et al., 2013), such traits are hypothesized to evolve as the by-product of sexual selection acting on the elaborate traits of males (Lande, 1980). The focus on mate choice, however, may be too narrow (Tarvin and Murphy, 2012), and the evolution of female ornamentation may be best considered under the broader context of social selection since females often compete not only for access to mates 91 and breeding opportunities, but also for non-sexual resources (West-Eberhard, 1983; Tobias et al., 2012). Although outcompeting conspecifics for limited breeding opportunities or resources increases the reproductive success of females, traits that enhance female competition also may be costly in terms of lower offspring quality (Rosvall, 2011b). Ornamental traits that evolve by sexual and/or social selection should honestly signal the quality of their bearer. Signal honesty may be maintained by the physiological costs of producing and maintaining ornamental traits so that only those individuals of high quality are able to display the most elaborate form of such traits (reviewed in Tibbetts, 2014). Since females usually provide the bulk of resources for the production of offspring, they may be faced with resource trade-offs between producing ornamentation and offspring (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995). For example, females that invest resources, such as carotenoids, into the production of elaborate ornaments have been observed to produce poor-quality eggs (Morales et al., 2009). Consequently, males of mutually ornamented species may not benefit from choosing the most elaborately ornamented females as mates if their offspring are lower quality, and thus a negative relationship between female ornamentation and offspring quality is interpreted as support for the genetic correlation hypothesis (Nordeide et al., 2013). While this explanation may be limited to species where allocation of resources to the production of ornamentation and offspring coincide during the breeding season and/or male mate choice has been shown to operate (reviewed in Nordeide et al., 2013), it is also important to consider alternative explanations that may arise by social mechanisms that maintain signal honesty. For example, enlarging the size of forehead patch of male collared flycatchers (Ficedula albicollis) increased competition among males, and yearling males displaying enlarged forehead patches spent more time involved in competition and as a result, 92 provisioned their nestlings less compared to control males (Qvarnström, 1997). Consequently, social costs of dishonest signalling may lower parental investment (Qvarnström, 1997) or reduce the resources available to invest in reproduction (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995), and may result in poorer quality offspring; however, as far as I am aware, no study has manipulated female ornamentation prior to breeding and explored whether social costs of possessing elaborate ornamentation influence offspring quality. In the current study, my aim was to test whether variation in plumage brightness of female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) influences offspring quality. Female tree swallows display dorsal plumage that ranges from dull brown to bright iridescent blue-green. Dull brown plumage is predominately displayed by young females in their first breeding season (i.e., second year females (SY); Hussell, 1983), whereas older females (i.e., aftersecond-year; ASY) display ‘male-like’ iridescent blue-green plumage. The delayed plumage maturation of SY females has been shown to function as a signal of low competitive ability thereby reducing the amount of aggression received from ornamented ASY females (Coady and Dawson, 2013). ASY females that are more ornamented, and presumably higher quality because they lay heavier eggs and fledge more offspring, display plumage that is brighter, with greater ultraviolet (UV) and blue chroma, and reflects light maximally at shorter wavelengths (i.e., bluer; Bitton et al., 2008; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Although it is currently unknown whether bright plumage of ASY females functions as a status signal in tree swallows, Bitton et al. (2008) reported positive assortative mating for plumage brightness, which may result either from mutual mate choice or competition among females for males with a nest site. Since male tree swallows do not alter their investment in parental care or mating strategies in relation to experimentally altered plumage brightness of females 93 (Berzins and Dawson, 2016; Chapter 4), it is unlikely that bright plumage is a signal of attractiveness that is preferred by males and assortative mating likely results from bright females having greater competitive ability. Previous studies in tree swallows also have shown that more ornamented females have greater levels of nest parasitism, poorer immune defences, lower hematocrit levels and produce offspring that are smaller or in poor condition (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). This negative relationship between female ornamentation and offspring quality could be interpreted as evidence in support of the genetic correlation hypothesis (Nordeide et al., 2013); however, more ornamented female tree swallows may experience social costs as a result of conspecifics challenging their status (Coady, 2011; Chapter 3), thereby resulting in poorer quality offspring produced by more ornamented females. I tested whether plumage brightness of female tree swallows influences offspring quality by experimentally enhancing and reducing the plumage brightness of females relative to controls (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). I previously reported evidence of social control of signal honesty in tree swallows (Chapter 3); females in the enhanced and control plumage brightness treatments were less likely to retain their nest site following the plumage manipulation than females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment. Moreover, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment initiated their clutches later than females in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments, which is consistent with social costs imposed on females dishonestly signalling high quality and engaging in frequent and aggressive agonistic interactions with naturally bright conspecific females (e.g., like-versuslike aggression, Senar, 2006). Consequently, females whose signals are challenged by conspecifics may spend more time involved in agonistic interactions and/or have fewer 94 resources to invest in self-maintenance and parental care; therefore, I predicted that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment would produce offspring of lower quality than females in the control treatment. I also predicted that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment, since they may have been involved in agonistic interactions with conspecifics signalling lower quality or fewer interactions altogether (Chapter 3), would produce offspring of higher quality than females in the control and enhanced plumage brightness treatments. Following the manipulation of plumage brightness, all nests at my study area were monitored (Chapter 3) and nestling growth, size, and mass were measured to estimate quality (see Dawson et al., 2005). I also sexed all nestlings so that differences in size, mass and growth between males and females could be controlled statistically (Rosivall et al., 2009). 5.3. Material and methods 5.3.1. Study area and general field methods Tree swallows were studied while breeding in nest boxes near Prince George BC, Canada (53ºN, 123ºW) from May to August in 2010 and 2011. The study area consisted of open agricultural areas and small wetlands that were intermixed with patches of coniferous and deciduous trees (see Dawson et al., 2005 for more details). I began visiting nest boxes daily in early May to document nest construction; once a nest box contained a nest with a completed nest cup, I captured females in the nest box (Chapter 3). ASY females were sequentially assigned to a plumage brightness treatment by capture order (Chapter 3), and the brightness of their plumage was experimentally altered as described in Berzins and Dawson (2016). Briefly, I enhanced plumage brightness by applying non-toxic permanent blue marker, and reduced by applying silicon paste evenly to the dorsal feathers; control females 95 were treated in a similar matter, except that I used a marker containing water. Prior to manipulation I found no differences in age, body mass and plumage characteristics of females among plumage brightness treatments (Chapter 3). In total, the plumage brightness of 61 females was experimentally altered (enhanced = 30, reduced = 31), while the plumage brightness of 28 females were in the control group. Following the experimental alteration of female plumage brightness, I monitored nest sites to document clutch initiation and completion dates (Chapter 3). Beginning twelve days after clutch completion, I checked nests daily to record the hatching of eggs, designated as day 0 of the brood-rearing period. On day 4, the legs of each nestling in a brood were uniquely marked with a non-toxic marker for individual identification, and I measured mass with a spring balance (nearest 0.125 g) and the combined length of the head and bill (hereafter head-bill, nearest 0.1 mm). After the day 4 measurement, I subsequently measured mass and length of the head-bill every two days from day 6 to 16. Starting on day 8, I measured the length of the ninth primary flight feather with a ruler (nearest 0.5 mm) and continued measurements every two days until day 16. On day 12, I collected a small blood sample (20 L) from the brachial vein of nestlings with non-heparinized capillary tubes for molecular sexing; blood samples were stored in Queen’s lysis buffer (Seutin et al., 1991) at -20 oC until DNA extraction. Any eggs that failed to hatch or nestlings found dead prior to day 12 were collected and stored at -20 oC until tissue collection and DNA extraction. I visited nests on day 22 to record the number of young that successfully fledged. 5.3.2. Molecular sexing To determine the sex of offspring, I amplified DNA extracted from blood and tissue samples (see Chapter 4) using the P2 and P8 primers, which have been used previously to 96 determine the sex of nestling tree swallows (Whittingham and Dunn, 2000). DNA extracted from blood samples was amplified using polymerase chain reaction (PCR) performed in a 10 µL final volume containing 1x PCR buffer, 0.2 mM of each dNTP (Invitrogen #10297-018), 2.5 mM MgCl2, 0.4 g/L bovine serum albumin (BSA; New England Biolabs #B90015), 0.3 M of P2 and P8 primers, 0.05 U/L Taq polymerase (Invitrogen #10342-020), and 20– 50 ng of genomic DNA. PCR was performed in a MJ Research Peltier Thermal Cycler under the following PCR cycling protocol: an initial denaturing step at 94 °C for 5 min, followed by 35 cycles of 94 °C for 30 sec, 47 °C for 60 sec. and 72 °C for 60 sec. A final extension cycle at 72 °C for 5 min completed the PCR. For DNA extracted from tissue samples of unhatched eggs (see Chapter 6), PCR reactions were performed using a Qiagen Multiplex PCR kit (Qiagen #206143) following the manufacturer’s instructions, except I used a final volume of 10 L and 5 L of Multiplex PCR Master Mix; primer concentrations and annealing temperatures remained the same as described above. The P2 and P8 primers amplify the CHD-W and CHD-Z genes located on the avian sex chromosomes W and Z (Griffiths et al., 1998). Since females are the heterogametic sex, the sex of each individual can be determined based on the number of PCR products amplified (i.e., when visualizing the gel, females are indicated by the presence of two bands, ZW, whereas males are indicated by a single band, ZZ; Griffiths et al., 1998). In tree swallows, the two bands for females are difficult to separate by electrophoresis because they are similar in size and so the PCR products first need to be digested using the restriction enzyme HaeIII (Whittingham and Dunn, 2000). Following Whittingham and Dunn (2000), I digested 5 µL of the PCR product with 10 U of HaeIII (Invitrogen # 15205-016), 1 µL of 10x buffer (provided with the HaeIII), and 5 µL water. The digest mixture was incubated at 37 °C for 1 97 hour, and a 5.4 µL aliquot was added to 0.6 µL of 10x loading buffer (provided with the HaeIII), and then added to a 1.5 % agarose gel stained with ethidium bromide. The digested PCR products were separated by electrophoresis and the gel was visualized under UV light to determine the sex of offspring. HaeIII cuts the CHD-Z fragment, but not the CHD-W fragment specific to females so females are identified by an individual having two bands, and males by a single band (Whittingham and Dunn 2000). I included an adult male and female tree swallow of known sex in every PCR and HaeIII digestion, and on every gel; in all cases the sex recorded when visualizing the gel matched the known sex of the adult. 5.3.3. Statistical analysis I used random intercept linear mixed models (LMM; lme4 package, Bates et al., 2015) to examine whether mass and lengths of ninth primary feathers and head-bill on day 16, as well as growth rates of nestlings, were influenced by the plumage brightness treatment of the female. Growth rate constants of individual nestlings were calculated using a linear model for ninth primary, Gompertz model for head-bill, and a logistic model for mass following Dawson et al. (2005), using only nestlings that had complete growth measures (i.e., nestlings measured every two days from day 4 to 16 (mass and head-bill) or day 8 to 16 (ninth primary feather)). For these models, either size or mass at day 16, or growth rate constant was the dependent variable, and plumage brightness treatment, year, and nestling sex (male or female) were included as fixed factors. Brood size on day 16 and time of measurement were included as covariates in models examining nestling size or mass at day 16, but only brood size was included as a covariate in models examining growth rate constants. Hatching date was not included in models because it was negatively related to time of measurement (r = -0.36, P = 0.02). Brood identity was included as a random factor to 98 account for the lack of independence among nestlings reared within the same brood and female identity as a random factor to account for the multiple observations for some females (N = 2). P-values for these LMM were calculated using the R package lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2016), and I removed non-significant covariates and factors that did not approach significance (P ≥ 0.10) in a backwards, stepwise fashion, but always retained plumage brightness treatment. To test whether fledging success differed by the plumage brightness of the female, I used a generalized linear model (GLM) fitted with binomial errors and logit link function for proportion data (Crawley, 2013). The response variable was the number of offspring that fledged and the binomial denominator was the original size of the brood. I included plumage brightness treatment and year as fixed factors, and hatching date, standardized to a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1 for experimental nests separately for each year, as a covariate. Since the residual deviance was greater than the degrees of freedom, I refitted the model with a quasi-binomial error structure to account for overdispersion (Crawley, 2013). I tested the significance of covariates and factors by assessing the change in deviance between models. Results were considered significant at P ≤ 0.05, and means are presented + 1 standard error (SE). The overall significance of omnibus tests was examined using post-hoc tests with a false discovery rate adjustment for multiple comparisons (Verhoeven et al., 2005). Parameter estimates (B) and effect sizes are reported, and effect sizes for post-hoc tests were calculated as the correlation coefficient (r) following Field et al. (2012). All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM Corp., 2011) and R (R Development Core Team, 2015) statistical software. 99 5.4. Results There was a tendency for length of the head-bill of nestlings to differ by the plumage brightness treatment of the female (F2, 36.91 = 3.10, P = 0.057) and by the sex of nestlings (F1, 169.83 = 26.51, P < 0.0001); however, the residuals of the model were not normal (Shapiro- Wilk normality test, P < 0.0001) and appeared to be influenced by three nestlings (out of 197) with small lengths of head-bill. After removing these nestlings from the model, the residuals were normal (Shapiro-Wilk normality test, P = 0.14); further, the results showed that length of head-bill differed significantly by the plumage brightness treatment of the female (Table 5.1; Figure 5.1). Post-hoc tests showed that nestlings reared in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment had shorter head-bills than nestlings reared in the control (B = -0.42 ± 0.20, t33.21 = -2.12, P = 0.05, r = 0.34, Figure 5.1) and reduced plumage brightness treatments (B = -0.59 ± 0.19, t37.94 = -3.03, P = 0.007, r = 0.44, Figure 5.1). No difference in length of head-bills of nestlings was detected between the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments (B = 0.16 ± 0.19, t38.86 = 0.89, P = 0.38, r = 0.14). Male nestlings had longer head-bills at day 16 than female nestlings (B = 0.43 ± 0.08, Table 5.1). Length of ninth primary flight feathers of nestlings did not differ by female plumage brightness treatment, but did vary annually (Table 5.1); nestlings had shorter ninth primaries in 2011 compared to 2010 (B = -4.53 ±1.64). Nestlings were also of similar mass on day 16 regardless of the plumage brightness treatment of the female (Table 5.1), but male nestlings were heavier than females (B = 1.00 ± 0.27; Table 5.1). Growth of nestling ninth primary feathers and head-bills were similar among plumage brightness treatments, but ninth primary feathers grew faster in 2010 than in 2011 (B = 0.50 ± 0.16; Table 5.1). There was some suggestion that mass growth rate constants of 100 nestlings differed by plumage brightness treatment (Table 5.1, Figure 5.2). Post-hoc tests showed that nestlings reared in broods in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment tended to gain mass at a slower rate than nestlings reared in control broods (B = -0.06 ± 0.03, t38.15 = -2.37, P = 0.05, r = 0.34, Figure 5.2), but not nestlings reared in the reduced plumage brightness treatment (B = -0.04 ± 0.02, t40.12 = -1.47, P = 0.21, r = 0.22, Figure 5.2). Mass gain was similar among nestlings reared in reduced and control plumage brightness treatments (B = -0.02 ± 0.02, t39.59 = -0.97, P = 0.33, r = 0.15, Figure 5.2). Fledging success of nestlings tended to decline with later hatching dates (B = -0.62 ± 0.36, F1, 46 = 3.12, P = 0.08), and was lower in 2011 than 2010 (B = -1.79 ± 0.47, F1, 47 = 14.56, P = 0.0004), but did not differ by the plumage brightness treatment of the female (F2, 48 = 1.07, P = 0.35). 5.5. Discussion I manipulated the plumage of female tree swallows to test whether variation in plumage brightness influenced nestling quality. Consistent with my predictions, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment produced nestlings with shorter head-bills, so they were structurally smaller, compared to nestlings in both the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments (Figure 5.1). There was also a tendency for nestlings reared by females with enhanced plumage brightness to gain mass more slowly than controls (Figure 5.2), but despite this, they were no lighter on day 16 than nestlings reared in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments (Table 5.1). I also predicted that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment would produce nestlings of higher quality (structurally larger, heavier, or faster growth) than nestlings reared by control females if they were involved in agonistic interactions with low-quality females or fewer interactions overall 101 (Chapter 3), but I observed no difference in offspring quality between the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments (Figure 5.1). This may suggest that although females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment were challenged less or by low-quality conspecifics, engaging in agonistic interactions is costly and reduces the amount of time or resources females can invest in reproduction (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995; Qvarnström, 1997). Alternatively, females may have been involved in fewer agonistic interactions, but their nestlings were not higher quality than controls because males invested less in parental care. Such differential allocation has been demonstrated previously in other species (Roulin, 1999; Mahr et al., 2012), but I believe this is unlikely to explain my results since male tree swallows do not provision their nestlings less when mated to a female whose plumage brightness was reduced (Berzins and Dawson, 2016). Previous studies in tree swallows have shown that nestlings produced by more ornamented females were smaller or in poorer condition (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Coady (2011) hypothesized that if more ornamented females are perceived as higher status than less-ornamented females, then social costs experienced by more ornamented females whose signal quality was challenged repeatedly by conspecifics may explain why they produced smaller offspring. Although it is currently unknown whether plumage brightness of female tree swallows is a signal of status, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment manipulated to display high-quality signals were less able to retain their nest site than females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment, suggesting they were challenged more frequently and aggressively by conspecifics (Chapter 3), and females with enhanced plumage brightness that did retain their nest site, bred later in the season (Chapter 3) and produced structurally smaller offspring (Figure 5.1). My results for 102 female tree swallows are similar to those previously reported for yearling male collared flycatchers; males manipulated to dishonestly display an enlarged forehead patch were less likely to establish a breeding territory, and those that did fed their nestlings less than controls (Qvarnström, 1997). Although I have no data on parental provisioning rates, length of nestling head-bills on day 4 did not differ by plumage brightness treatment (P = 0.96, Berzins, unpublished data), which may suggest that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment provisioned their nestlings at lower rates and/or provided lower quality food. Lower investment in nestlings by females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment is consistent with ornament-fecundity (Fitzpatrick et al., 1995) or ornamentparental care (Qvarnström, 1997) trade-offs, although I am unable to determine which mechanism led to the lower offspring quality observed in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment. Nevertheless, females displaying enhanced plumage brightness likely had fewer resources or less time to invest in rearing nestlings, without jeopardizing their own selfmaintenance, as a result of engaging in agonistic interactions with high-quality conspecifics. Thus, although elaborate ornamentation may benefit females, for example, in competition with conspecifics for quality mates or nest sites (Bitton et al., 2008), social costs associated with the possession of ornamentation may lower female fecundity (Chapter 3) and offspring quality (Figure 5.1) and should be considered in future studies. Although my results for lower nestling quality are consistent with brighter females experiencing social costs, there are also other non-mutually exclusive explanations for my results. First, nestlings in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have been smaller than nestlings in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments simply because they hatched later in the season (P < 0.045, Chapter 3). Later hatched nestlings have lower 103 survival prospects (Dawson, 2008), and so parents may strategically reduce their reproductive effort to invest in their own self-maintenance and enhance future survival and fecundity rather than investing in offspring of low value (Winkler and Allen, 1996). It is important to note, however, that the delayed breeding by females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment appears to be a consequence of increased agonistic interactions with conspecifics (Chapter 3). In addition, if females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment were perceived as more attractive to potential mates and had greater opportunities to engage in extra-pair copulations, then males paired to attractive females may have been less certain of their paternity and provisioned nestlings less. This explanation, however, is also unlikely since females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment had a similar proportion of extra-pair offspring in their broods as did females in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments (Chapter 4), and there is also no evidence that male tree swallows provide lower investment in parental care when their brood contains extra-pair offspring (Whittingham et al., 1993). Finally, the smaller structural size of nestlings may have been due to females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment depositing a greater concentration of testosterone in the yolks of their eggs as a result of nest-site intrusions (Whittingham and Schwabl, 2002) since they likely experienced more frequent and aggressive challenges by naturally bright females of greater competitive ability (see Chapter 3). Although elevated yolk testosterone levels have been shown to enhance the growth of nestlings in some species (Eising et al., 2001; Müller et al., 2010), negative effects on nestling structural size have also been demonstrated (Sockman and Schwabl, 2000), and the effect of androgens on nestling development may depend on the concentration deposited by the female (Muriel et al., 2015). Unfortunately, the only way to distinguish whether the effect 104 of plumage brightness treatment on nestling quality is due to the effects of androgen deposited in egg yolks or lower provisioning rates by females would be to cross-foster nestlings among the plumage brightness treatments so that nestlings were reared by females other than their genetic mother. Interestingly, I only detected an effect of plumage brightness treatment on nestling structural size, but not body mass or length of ninth primary flight feathers of nestlings (Table 5.1). It is possible that nestling mass and feather length was maintained because males mated to females whose plumage brightness was enhanced increased their investment in parental care (Burley, 1988) and this at least partially compensated for any negative effect of plumage brightness treatment on nestling quality; however, given that a previous study reported no evidence of differential allocation by male tree swallows in relation to experimentally altered plumage brightness of females (Berzins and Dawson, 2016), this seems doubtful. Alternatively, an effect of plumage brightness treatment only on nestling structural size may provide support to the above suggestion that the costs of engaging in agnostic interactions did in fact influence female provisioning behaviour. For example, females may have provisioned nestlings at an adequate rate to maintain the energy requirements for tissue development, but nestlings may have received insufficient dietary minerals, such as calcium, for skeletal growth (Dawson et al., 2005). Previously, a study in tree swallows demonstrated that calcium-fed nestlings were structurally larger, heavier and had longer ninth primary feathers than control nestlings (Dawson and Bidwell, 2005). Such results suggest that parents may face a trade-off between provisioning rate and searching for food items containing calcium (reviewed in Dawson and Bidwell, 2005), and consequently, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have had fewer resources or less 105 time available to invest in searching for shells and other sources of calcium. Nestlings that are structurally smaller at day 16 may be smaller-sized adults or fledge without their skeletal structure being fully developed, which may lower post fledging survival if they are weaker, poorer fliers, or have a greater risk of predation (reviewed in Tilgar et al., 2004). Moreover, dietary intake during development may influence the brightness of feathers grown, as demonstrated in male nestling eastern bluebirds (Sialia sialis; Doyle and Siefferman, 2014); however, it is unclear whether such effects would persist into adult life since feathers are molted and regrown annually and structural plumage coloration may be more sensitive to stress experienced during molt rather than nutritional condition (Peters et al., 2011). Unfortunately, recruitment of nestling tree swallows in my study area is very low (e.g., 0.05 probability of recruitment, Weegman et al., unpublished data), and so I am unable to determine whether apparent effects of plumage brightness treatment on nestling skeletal growth also had long-lasting effects on their plumage ornamentation and reproductive success as adults. Overall, my finding that females manipulated to signal enhanced plumage brightness produced lower quality nestlings is consistent with previous studies in tree swallows (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). I argue that this finding is not consistent with the genetic correlation hypothesis (sensu Nordeide et al., 2013) to explain the evolution of ornamental plumage of female tree swallows, but rather suggests that ornamented females may experience social costs by the mechanisms that ensure the honesty of quality signals. Despite such costs, however, female tree swallows may benefit from their ornamented plumage because they mate with males that have bright plumage (Bitton et al. 2008), which may increase the reproductive value of sons. For example, male tree swallows with bright 106 plumage sire a greater number of offspring than dull males (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016), and if male offspring inherit the attractive traits of their father (e.g., ‘sexy son’ hypothesis, Weatherhead and Robertson, 1979), then these ‘sexy sons’ would become ‘sexy fathers’ for which evidence of female preference exists (Bitton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2016). Given that there is there is little evidence to suggest male mate choice in this mutually ornamented species (e.g., Berzins and Dawson, 2016; Chapter 4), positive assortative mating for plumage brightness (Bitton et al., 2008) is likely due to females with bright plumage having greater competitive ability. Future studies that examine the evolution of plumage brightness of female tree swallows should evaluate if this trait is also beneficial to females in a non-sexual context, such as a status signal in competition for resources during the non-breeding season (e.g., Tarvin and Murphy, 2012). 107 Table 5.1. Results of random intercept linear mixed models comparing mass and size at day 16, and growth rates of nestling tree swallows reared in broods where female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced, or remained unchanged (controls). F df P Nestling size Head-bill (mm) Plumage brightness treatment Sex 4.75 33.15 2, 36.49 1, 164.22 0.01 <0.0001 Ninth primary feather (mm) Plumage brightness treatment Year 0.21 7.58 2, 48.63 1, 50.80 0.81 0.008 Mass (g) Plumage brightness treatment Year Time of measurement Sex 0.14 3.49 3.57 13.48 2, 28.87 1, 26.08 1, 35.27 1, 169.07 0.87 0.07 0.07 <0.001 Nestling growth Head-bill Plumage brightness treatment Sex Brood size 1.65 3.39 3.31 2, 38.25 1, 171.51 1, 55.56 0.20 0.07 0.07 Ninth primary Plumage brightness treatment Year 0.13 9.38 2, 48.30 1, 51.28 0.88 0.003 Mass Plumage brightness treatment 2.87 2, 39.29 0.068 108 Figure 5.1. Mean (± SE) length of the combined head and bill of nestling tree swallows at day 16 in broods where female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. Sample sizes indicate the number of broods in each treatment, and are given above error bars. 109 Figure 5.2. Mean (± SE) growth rate constant for mass of nestling tree swallows reared in broods where female plumage brightness was experimentally reduced or enhanced compared to controls. Sample sizes indicate the number of broods in each treatment, and are given above error bars. See Methods for calculation of growth rate constants. 110 Chapter 6: Experimentally altering exposure to testosterone and its estrogenic metabolites prior to breeding reduces extra-pair paternity in female tree swallows 6.1. Abstract Extra-pair paternity occurs commonly in many socially monogamous animal species, yet the reasons why females mate with males outside the social pair bond remain poorly understood. Evidence from previous studies suggests that individual differences in behavioural traits may underlie variation in the rate of extra-pair paternity; however, it remains to be determined whether behavioural traits and promiscuity are mediated by the same hormone(s). Since testosterone (T) and its estrogen metabolites mediate aggressive and sexual behaviours, I tested whether such hormones also influence extra-pair copulation behaviour by experimentally elevating T using implants containing T, and blocking the estrogenic and androgenic actions of T using implants containing 1,4,6-androstatrien-3, 17dione in combination with flutamide (ATD+F) in female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Females with sham implants served as controls. Consistent with my predictions, ATD+F females produced fewer extra-pair offspring compared to control females. This result suggests that blocking the estrogenic and/or androgenic actions of T influences extrapair copulation behaviour of females. Contrary to my predictions, broods of T-treated females also contained fewer extra-pair offspring compared to controls. This result is consistent with previous studies and suggests that increasing exposure to T does not increase the propensity of females to seek extra-pair matings. Overall, my study provides evidence that altering exposure to T and its estrogenic metabolites during pre-breeding influences whether females engage in extra-pair copulations, and provides insight into the role of 111 proximate mechanisms, such as hormone-mediated behaviour, in driving variation in rates of extra-pair paternity in birds. 6.2. Introduction In many socially monogamous animal species, females commonly engage in extrapair copulations, which may result in offspring being sired by males other than a female’s social mate (Griffith et al., 2002). Since females may incur costs by copulating with males outside of the social pair bond, this behaviour is hypothesized to have evolved because females obtain fitness benefits from extra-pair mating (i.e., direct and indirect benefits; reviewed in Birkhead and Møller, 1992; Petrie and Kempenaers, 1998). Alternatively, females may engage in extra-pair copulations as a result of genetic constraints, such as extrapair copulation behaviour of females evolving as a correlated response to selection for fecundity or other behaviours expressed by females (reviewed in Forstmeier et al. 2014). Rates of extra-pair paternity in socially monogamous birds vary widely among species and populations, and even within populations there is considerable variation; some females have mixed-paternity broods while others do not (Ryder et al., 2012; Patrick et al., 2012; Varian-Ramos and Webster, 2012; Hsu et al., 2015). Variation in rates of extra-pair paternity among females may be due to intrinsic differences in behavioural traits, such as exploratory (van Oers et al., 2008; but see Patrick et al., 2012) or sexual (Forstmeier, 2007) behaviour. These individual differences in behavioural traits, commonly referred to as animal ‘personalities’(Carere et al., 2005; Dingemanse et al., 2010; Réale et al., 2010a), are generally consistent over time and across contexts, such that some individuals tend to be consistently more aggressive, bold or exploratory than other individuals (Sih et al., 2004a). As such, these traits may confer an advantage to females in some contexts, but may also be 112 maladaptive in other contexts. For example, aggressive female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are more successful at acquiring limited nesting sites (Rosvall, 2008), but they also produce offspring of poor quality because aggressive females provide lower levels of maternal care (Rosvall, 2011b). Nevertheless, females that display highly aggressive behaviour may have a greater propensity to engage in extra-pair copulations, as demonstrated previously in lizards (Egernia whitii) for which aggressive females produced a greater number of offspring sired by extra-pair males (While et al., 2009). Therefore, individual differences in personality of females may underlie variation in extra-pair paternity. Personality traits may be related to promiscuous behaviour if they are mediated by the same hormone(s) and as a result are correlated in their expression (hormone pleiotropy; (Ketterson and Nolan, 1999). In female birds, concentrations of circulating hormones such as testosterone (T) and 17β-estradiol (E2) peak prior to egg laying (e.g., Ketterson et al., 2005; Williams et al., 2004), which coincides with an increase in aggressive (Cristol and Johnsen, 1994) and copulatory behaviour (Barber and Robertson, 2007; Crowe et al., 2009). Aggressive behaviour has been shown to increase a female’s success at obtaining limited nest sites (Rosvall 2008) and as such may influence extra-pair copulation behaviour by enabling females to invade neighbouring territories in search of extra-pair mates. In addition, sexual behaviour of females may enhance the opportunity for engaging in extra-pair copulations; for example, during the fertile period, female humans (Homo sapiens) have greater desire or interest in extra-pair mates (e.g., Gangestad et al., 2002; Gangestad et al., 2005). Experimentally elevating T concentrations increases aggressive behaviour in female birds (Zysling et al., 2006; Sandell, 2007; Rosvall, 2013) and sexual interest, libido and behaviour in humans (reviewed in Bolour and Braunstein, 2005); however, studies 113 investigating whether elevated T mediates extra-pair copulation behaviour in female birds have produced mixed results. T-treated female dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) did not differ from control females in the proportion of extra-pair offspring in their nests (Gerlach and Ketterson, 2013), whereas extra-pair paternity was lower for T-treated female spotless starlings (Sturnus unicolor) and blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) when compared to control females (García-Vigón et al., 2008; de Jong, 2013, respectively). Furthermore, de Jong (2013) treated a third group of female blue tits with flutamide (F), which blocks androgen receptors, and found that the number of nests containing extra-pair young did not differ between F-treated and control females. The results of these experimental studies suggest that elevated levels of T may not play a role in mediating the mating behaviour of females; however, none of these investigations manipulated T by inhibiting the conversion of T to its estrogenic metabolites. For example, the effects of T on extra-pair copulation behaviour may occur through an alternative mechanism, such as the aromatization of T to E2. Experimental studies have demonstrated that decreasing levels of E2 lowers sexual behaviour of female birds, such as receptivity and solicitation (reviewed in Riters and Alger, 2011), and blocking the estrogenic actions of T, in addition to androgen receptors, is necessary to fully reduce aggressive behaviour (Archawaranon and Wiley, 1988). Therefore, the use of an aromatase inhibitor to suppress the conversion of T to E2 in combination with F is necessary to fully understand the effects of T on the mating behaviour of females. My aim was to examine whether T and its estrogen metabolites mediate behavioural traits that influence a female’s propensity to engage in extra-pair copulations and the consequences for their reproductive success, using tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) as a study species. Tree swallows exhibit high rates of extra-pair paternity, with up to 85 % of 114 nests containing at least one extra-pair young (O’Brien and Dawson 2007). Females in this species engage in extra-pair copulations with males from neighbouring territories (Venier et al., 1993), and presumably at roost sites away from the nest (sensu Dunn and Whittingham, 2005; Stapleton and Robertson, 2006). Both males and females initiate extra-pair copulations, but those initiated by the female are more likely to result in fertilization (Venier et al. 1993). Forced extra-pair copulations from males are rare (e.g., see Venier et al. 1993), and to protect their paternity within the brood, males copulate frequently with their social mate (Crowe et al., 2009). Female tree swallows are also highly aggressive in competition with conspecifics for, and in defence of, a nest site (Leffelaar and Robertson 1985; Rosvall 2008). As such, hormones that mediate the sexual and aggressive behaviour of female tree swallows may also influence the pursuit and acceptance of extra-pair copulations. I tested the effects of T and its estrogenic metabolites on extra-pair paternity in tree swallows by experimentally elevating concentrations of T using implants containing exogenous T (increasing T) and blocking the estrogenic and androgenic actions of T using implants containing 1,4,6-androstatrien-3, 17-dione (ATD) and flutamide (F) in combination (ATD+F; decreasing the effects of T and E2); control females received empty implants. As far as I am aware, the combined use of ATD+F has not been employed previously to examine how T-mediated behaviour influences extra-pair paternity in female birds. If extrapair copulation behaviour of female tree swallows is correlated with the expression of aggressive or sexual behaviour and is mediated by T and its estrogenic metabolites, then I predicted that T-treated females would have a greater proportion of extra-pair offspring in their broods than females in the ATD+F and control treatments as a result of being more aggressive (Rosvall 2013) or having greater sexual interest in extra-pair mates. In contrast, I 115 predicted that females treated with ATD+F would produce fewer extra-pair offspring than Ttreated and control females as a result of being less aggressive (e.g., Hau et al., 2000; Canoine and Gwinner, 2002) or having lower sexual interest or receptivity towards extra-pair mates. Following the experimental alteration of T and its estrogenic metabolites, I subsequently used microsatellite markers to assign parentage to offspring and determined the proportion of extra-pair paternity in nests. I also monitored the nests of females during the breeding season to examine how the treatment influenced reproductive success. 6.3. Material and methods 6.3.1. Study area and species I studied tree swallows breeding in nest boxes west of Prince George, BC, Canada (53ºN, 123ºW), from May to August 2012. The study area consisted of open agricultural areas intermixed with small wetlands and patches of coniferous and deciduous trees (see Dawson et al., 2005 for more details). Tree swallows are small, migratory, aerial insectivores that are socially monogamous and exhibit high rates of extra-pair paternity (e.g., up to 85 % of broods contain at least one extra-pair offspring, see O’Brien and Dawson, 2007 and references therein). Clutch size is typically 4-7 eggs, which is incubated only by the female, and both sexes contribute to provisioning offspring (Winkler et al., 2011). 6.3.2. General field methods During early May, I visited nests daily to document nest building, and captured females in nest boxes once construction of the nest cup was complete, but prior to eggs being laid. From a subset of females I collected a blood sample (approx. 50–80 µL) from the brachial vein to measure pre-laying levels of T and E2, and recorded handling time (elapsed 116 time from closing the trap to collection of the blood sample) using a stopwatch (mean ± SE: 4.16 ± 0.35 min, range: 2.78–9.35, N = 22 females). Blood samples were collected in heparinized capillary tubes and stored on ice until they were centrifuged for 5 minutes in the lab. Plasma was transferred to micro-centrifuge tubes and stored at –80 oC until hormone analysis (details below). Age of females was determined according to plumage colour as being in either their second year (SY) of life or older (after-second year; ASY; Hussell 1983); only ASY females were used in this study because SY females tend to breed later than ASY females (Stutchbury and Robertson, 1988) and also may exhibit different strategies than ASY females in response to experimental manipulations (e.g., Ardia and Clotfelter, 2007). ASY females were sequentially allocated by capture order to the T, ATD+F, or control treatment after determining the treatment order randomly. Females received implants constructed from Silastic® laboratory tubing (1.47 mm I.D., 1.96 mm O.D.; Dow Corning #508-006) sealed at both ends with Silastic glue. For T implants, I used 6-mm implants packed with 3.5 mm of testosterone (approx. 0.0048 g; Sigma–Aldrich # T1500), a lower dose than Rosvall (2013), who reported hatching failure of eggs in the nests of female tree swallows implanted with 5 mm of T. I constructed ATD and F implants of equal size and these contained on average 0.0052 g of ATD (Steraloids, Inc #A4100-000) and 0.0035 g of F (Sigma–Aldrich #F9397). Implants from all treatments were cleaned with ethanol, and incubated at 37 oC overnight in tubes containing 0.9 % saline (Sigma-Aldrich #S8776) to begin the diffusion process of hormones across the tubing and avoid the surge of hormone release that can occur after implantation (Smith et al. 1977). Implants were then transferred to tubes containing fresh saline and transported to the field. In the field, I treated all females with a topical anesthetic 117 and then made a small incision using surgical scissors above the left flank. Two implants (T and sham, ATD and F, or two sham) were inserted subcutaneously under the skin along the flank and the incision site was sealed with cyanoacrylate tissue adhesive (VetbondTM #1469SB). I implanted a total of 12 T, 15 ATD+F, and 13 control females for this study (see Table 6.1). To examine whether experimentally altering exposure to T and its estrogenic metabolites influenced reproductive success of females, all nests boxes were monitored daily to record the date of clutch initiation (where January 1 = 1) and clutch size. Freshly laid eggs were numbered with a non-toxic marker for identification and were weighed with a digital scale (nearest 0.01 g). Once a clutch was complete, I checked nests daily to determine the onset of incubation, at which time nests were left undisturbed. None of the T-treated females incubated their clutches; these eggs were therefore collected 7 to 9 days after clutch completion and stored at –20 oC until DNA extraction. Nests in the ATD+F and control treatments were checked daily beginning 12 days after clutch completion to record the day of hatch, designated as day 0 of the brood-rearing period. Any eggs that failed to hatch from these nests were collected and stored as described above. On day 4 after hatching, I uniquely marked the legs of each nestling in a brood with a non-toxic marker for individual identification. I measured mass with a spring balance (nearest 0.125 g) and length of the combined head and bill (hereafter, head-bill) with digital calipers (nearest 0.1 mm) every 2 days from day 4 to 16, and length of the ninth primary feather with a ruler (nearest 0.5 mm) from day 8 to 16. On day 12, a small blood sample (20 L) was collected for paternity analysis from the brachial vein of nestlings and stored in Queen’s lysis buffer at 4 oC (Seutin et al., 1991) until DNA extraction (see details below). From nestlings found dead in the nest 118 prior to day 12, I collected brain tissue and stored it in the same manner as blood. On day 22, I visited nests to determine the number of nestlings that successfully fledged. I captured adult males while they were feeding offspring and collected a blood sample for paternity analysis. Control- and ATD+F-treated females were recaptured on day 12 to 16 of the nestling period to remove implants and collect blood samples to measure levels of T and E2 post-implants. I also recaptured T-treated females, and captured their social mates when possible, before collecting the clutch. Unfortunately, I was unable to capture the social mate at 5 broods where the female had been implanted with T. Blood samples drawn from experimental females to measure T and E2 concentrations were collected on average 3.58 minutes after capture (± 0.23 standard error (SE), range: 2.2–7.4 minutes, N = 22 females). 6.3.3. Hormone measurements I quantified concentrations of T in plasma samples of control and T-treated females using a T enzyme immunoassay (EIA) kit (Expanded Range Salivary Testosterone EIA kit, Salimetrics #1-2402). As ATD has a high cross-reactivity with the T assay kit (Vandermeer, 2013), I instead quantified E2 concentrations in plasma samples of females in the control and ATD+F treatment using an E2 EIA kit (High Sensitivity Salivary 17β-estradiol EIA, Salimetrics #1-3702). Both assay kits have been used previously to quantify T and E2 directly from avian plasma without extraction (Washburn et al., 2007; Hall and MacDougallShackleton, 2012) and were validated for use in tree swallows by demonstrating a displacement curve from a serial dilution of pooled plasma samples that was parallel to the standard curve for each assay. 119 For the T assay, plasma samples were diluted 1:1 with assay buffer (total volume of plasma was 12.5 µL). Due to low plasma volumes only five samples (all T-treated females) were run in duplicate; all other samples were assayed only once. The T kit had a high crossreactivity with other androgens, such as dihydrotestosterone (36.4 %), 19-nortestosterone (21.01 %), 11-hydroxytestosterone (1.90 %), and androstenedione (1.16 %), and a low crossreactivity (<0.5 %) with other hormones and compounds (see manufacturer’s instructions for more details). Given the high cross-reactivity of the T kit with the androgens listed above, I report my results from the T assay as ‘androgen concentrations’ as opposed to T concentrations. The volume of plasma used in the E2 assay ranged from 4 to 20 µL due to the availability of plasma, and E2 concentrations were corrected based on the dilution factor (range of dilutions: 1:4 to 1:20 in assay buffer). Most samples were assayed only once; two ATD+F and one control sample were assayed in duplicate. The sensitivity of the assay was 0.1 pg/mL (see manufacturer’s instructions) and the concentration of all samples was above this minimum detection limit. This assay had a low cross reactivity with estrone (1.23 %), estriol (0.23 %), ethynylestradiol (0.19 %), and prednisone (0.02 %), and had no crossreactivity with other hormones or compounds (see manufacturer’s instructions for more details). All samples were run in a single assay for each kit. The intra-assay coefficient of variation calculated from high and low controls of known concentration provided with each kit were 8.2 % and 1.4 % for T and 0.7 % and 1.2 % for E2 respectively. 120 6.3.4. Paternity analysis I removed the germinal disk or embryonic tissue from eggs that failed to hatch and stored it in Queen’s lysis buffer at 4 oC (Seutin et al. 1991) until DNA extraction. The germinal disk or embryonic tissue was not identified in three eggs (one egg from two clutches in the T treatment, and one egg from a clutch in the ATD+F treatment), which I considered as unfertilized for the paternity analysis. Genomic DNA from blood and tissue samples was extracted using DNeasy Blood & Tissue kits (Qiagen #69506), and from germinal disks using QIAamp DNA Micro kits (Qiagen #56304) following the manufacturer’s protocols. For 22 females in this experiment where blood was collected for hormone assays using heparin, I extracted genomic DNA from feather samples collected from the same birds as part of another study, following a slightly modified feather extraction protocol (protocol nine; Nishiguchi et al., 2002). Extracted DNA was amplified at either five or six microsatellite loci developed for Tachycineta swallows (Tle19, TaBi34, TaBi6, Tal8, TaBi10 and/or TaBi 8; Makarewich et al., 2009). Duplex polymerase chain reaction (PCR) was performed with two primer pairs TaBi6/Tle19 and Tal8/TaBi34, and PCR for TaBi10 and TaBi8 was carried out as single reactions. For DNA extracted from blood samples, the final 10 L volume for PCR contained 1x PCR buffer, 0.2 mM of each dNTP (Invitrogen #10297-018), 1.75–2.25 mM MgCl2, 0.4 g/L bovine serum albumin (BSA; New England Biolabs #B90015), 0.08–0.2 M of forward-labelled and reverse primers, 0.05 U/L Taq polymerase (Invitrogen #10342-020), and 20–50 ng of genomic DNA. PCR was performed in a MJ Research Peltier Thermal Cycler under the following PCR cycling protocol: an initial denaturing step at 95 °C for 4 min, followed by 35 cycles of 95 °C for 50 sec, 56–60 °C for 60 sec (56 °C for the duplex reactions and 60 °C for TaBi10 andTaBi8), and 65-72 °C for 90 121 sec (65 °C for the duplex reactions and 72 °C for TaBi10 andTaBi8). A final extension cycle at 65–72 °C for 5 min (65 °C for duplex reactions and 72 °C for TaBi10 andTaBi8) completed the PCR. For all other DNA, PCR reactions were performed using a Qiagen Multiplex PCR kit (Qiagen #206143) following manufacturer’s instructions, except I used a final volume of 10 L and 3–5 L of Multiplex PCR Master Mix. For DNA from germinal disks only, I added 0.4 g/L of BSA to the DNA and incubated the samples at 95 °C for 5 min prior to PCR. Primer concentrations and annealing temperatures remained the same as described above. PCR products were run on an ABI 3130xl automated sequencer (Applied Biosystems) to obtain individual genotypes and assign paternity. Data were analyzed blind to sample identity using Peak ScannerTM Software v1.0 (Applied Biosystems). Allele frequencies and exclusion probabilities for microsatellite loci in my study population were calculated for all adults using CERVUS 3.0 (Kalinowski et al., 2007; see Table 4.1). I genotyped 295 adults (resident females and males, and non-breeding individuals) and 1116 offspring as part of a larger three-year study (2010–2012). Individuals were genotyped at five loci Tle19, TaBi34, TaBi6, Tal8, and TaBi10; however, due to a high null allele frequency for Tal8 in my study population, I also genotyped all males and extrapair offspring that were homozygous at Tal8 plus the genetic mother at a sixth locus (TaBi8) for individuals sampled in 2010–2011. Starting in 2012, individuals were genotyped at six loci to ensure that parentage was assigned using five loci. TaBi8 significantly deviated from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (P = 0.002; Table 4.1), but this locus was never used as a single criterion to determine parentage. I classified offspring as within-pair if they matched the resident male at five loci, and extra-pair offspring if they mismatched the resident male at a minimum of 1 locus. For this experiment, only a single nestling mismatched the resident 122 male at only 1 locus, and this nestling shared alleles with a sibling that mismatched the resident male at 4 loci. All other mismatches among nestlings and resident males occurred at 2 or more loci. I was able to assign parentage using 5 loci with no apparent null alleles to all offspring except for four offspring genotyped at only four loci either due to null alleles or poor amplification. For these four offspring, I calculated the probability of chance inclusion following O’Brien and Dawson (2007). All four offspring were within-pair, and the probability that a randomly chosen male would match these offspring at all four loci was low (mean ± standard error, 0.0028 ± 0.0025; range: 0.0010 to 0.0064). 6.3.4. Statistical analysis To determine whether treating females with implants altered concentrations of androgens and E2, I used a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with timing of blood sample (pre-breeding or nestling rearing) as a within-subject repeated measure, treatment as a between-subject factor, and an interaction between timing of blood sampling and treatment. Since only a few females from each treatment were blood sampled both before and after receiving an implant (N = 3-5 females sampled at both times per treatment), I also used a one-way ANOVA to test for differences in T and E2 levels among treatment groups after females were implanted. To examine whether extra-pair paternity differed by treatment, I used a generalized linear model (GLM) fitted with binomial errors and logit link function for proportion data (Crawley 2013). The response variable was the number of within-pair offspring and the binomial denominator was the number of offspring in the brood. Treatment was included as a fixed factor, and the number of days a female was implanted prior to initiating a clutch (mean ± SE, 9.63 ± 1.35 days, range: 1–30) was included as a covariate. Clutch initiation 123 date and number of days implanted were correlated (r = 0.93, P < 0.0001), so I did not include clutch initiation date in the analysis. I tested the model for overdispersion by determining whether the residual deviance was greater than the degrees of freedom, and I refitted the model with a quasi-binomial error structure to account for overdispersion (Crawley, 2013). Following Crawley (2013), I tested the significance of the interaction and covariate by assessing the analysis of deviance between models. Four females initiated clutches the day following the manipulation (see below); I had paternity data for two (one T and one ATD+F) and included these data in the analysis. The T female laid the first egg (extra-pair offspring) and then delayed egg laying for 4 days; all subsequent offspring were within-pair offspring. The ATD+F treated female continued to lay eggs after being manipulated and exclusion of this female did not change the overall conclusions of my results. To test whether clutch initiation dates differed among females after receiving implants, I used a general least-squares model from the package nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2016) to account for heteroscedasticity in variances following Cleasby and Nakagawa (2011). From this analysis, I excluded three T and one ATD+F female that initiated a clutch the morning after receiving implants because the physiological processes associated with egg formation would have been initiated before these females were implanted, and so it is unlikely the hormones influenced initiation dates in these cases. The T-implanted females skipped egg laying for 2–4 days after initiating a clutch, while the ATD+F-implanted female continued to lay eggs; nevertheless all four females were included in subsequent analyses because the effects of silastic implants on T and aromatase activity can be detected within a day of being implanted (see Smith et al., 1977; Balthazart et al., 1990). I tested whether clutch size and 124 average egg mass differed among females in each treatment using separate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for each variable that included clutch initiation date as a covariate to control for the seasonal decline in clutch size that occurs in tree swallows (Winkler et al., 2014). I used a z-transformation to center and scale clutch initiation date so that the effect of treatment on clutch size or average egg mass could be interpreted in the presence of a treatment x initiation date interaction (Schielzeth, 2010). To determine whether nestling performance differed among the nests of females in the ATD+F and control treatments, I examined mass and size (length of ninth primary feather and head-bill) at day 16, and the growth rates of nestlings. Growth rate constants were calculated for each nestling using a logistic model for mass, Gompertz model for headbill, and linear model for ninth primary following Dawson et al. (2005). All nests had complete growth data, but one nest was measured on day 17 and was not included in the analysis of mass or size at day 16. Moreover, since I was interested in the effects of female treatment on nestling growth and size, I excluded from these analyses two nests (one ATD+F and one control) where the female was not observed after the hatching of eggs and a second (helper) female was provisioning the nestlings. I used random intercept linear mixed models (nlme, Pinheiro et al., 2016) with mass or size at day 16, or growth rate constant as dependent variables, treatment as the fixed factor, and brood identity as a random factor. I also z-transformed brood size at day 16 and time of measurement and included both variables as covariates in models analyzing mass and size at day 16, but only included brood size in models analyzing growth rate constants. I also included the interaction between treatment and brood size at day 16. When the residuals of models were not homoscedastic, I incorporated a variance function to account for heteroscedasticity (Cleasby and Nakagawa, 125 2011). I tested whether the fledging success of nestlings, i.e., number fledged in relation to clutch size, differed between the ATD+F and control treatments for broods that hatched at least one nestling using a (GLM) fitted with quasi-binomial errors and logit link function as described above. Treatment was included as a fixed factor, and brood size at day 16 and hatch date as covariates. For all analyses, I removed interactions and covariates that did not approach significance (P ≥ 0.10) in a backwards, stepwise fashion. Means are presented ± 1 SE, and results were considered significant at P ≤ 0.05. The overall significance of omnibus tests was examined using post-hoc tests with a false discovery rate adjustment for multiple comparisons (Verhoeven et al. 2005). Where appropriate, I report parameter estimates (B) and effect sizes; effect sizes for the repeated measures analysis as general eta squared (ηG2) following Lakens (2013) and for post-hoc comparisons as the correlation coefficient (r) following Field et al. (2012). All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM Corp. 2011) and R (R Development Core Team 2015) statistical software. 6.4. Results Within individual females, androgen concentrations differed before receiving implants during pre-breeding and after receiving implants when measured during the nestling period (F1, 6 = 31.95, P = 0.00l, ηG2 = 0.59, Figure 6.1a), and between the T and control treatments (F1, 6 = 10.679, P = 0.02, ηG2 = 0.56, Figure 6.1a). The interaction between treatment and implant time was also significant (F1, 6 =129.50, P < 0.0001, ηG2 = 0.85), and when I analyzed the data separately by treatment, androgen concentrations decreased in control females after receiving sham implants (F1, 3 = 15.39, P = 0.03, ηG2 = 0.48, Figure 126 6.1a), but increased in females after being implanted with T (F1, 3 = 155.31, P = 0.001, ηG2 = 0.87, Figure 6.1a). Since the number of females blood-sampled both before and after receiving implants was small (N = 4 for each treatment), I also compared circulating levels of androgens for all control and T-treated females that were captured, either pre-breeding or during the nestling period. There was no difference in androgen concentration between treatments prior to females receiving implants (F1, 7 = 12.84, P = 0.14, ηG2= 0.29, Figure 6.1a), but T-treated females had a higher concentration of androgens compared to control females during brood rearing (F1, 11 = 86.56, P < 0.0001, ηG2= 0.90, Figure 6.1a). The concentration of E2 decreased from pre-breeding to day 12 of the nestling period in females treated with sham and ATD+F implants (F1, 6 = 12.95, P = 0.01, ηG2 = 0.52); however, there was no effect of treatment on E2 concentration (F1, 6 = 0.07, P = 0.80, ηG2 = 0.006) and the interaction between time and treatment was also non-significant (F1, 6 = 0.44, P = 0.53, ηG2 = 0.04). Since only three control females were blood-sampled both before and after receiving implants, I also compared E2 concentrations between treatment groups postimplant. E2 concentration did not differ between ATD+F-treated and control females for blood samples on day 12 of the nestling period in June (F1, 11 = 0.14, P = 0.71, ηG2 = 0.01, Figure 6.1b). The proportion of extra-pair paternity differed significantly among treatment groups (F2, 24 = 4.09, P = 0.03, Figure 6.2). The number of days a female was implanted prior to initiating a clutch was not a significant predictor of extra-pair paternity and was removed from the model (B = 0.06 ± 0.06; F1, 23 = 1.11, P = 0.30). Post-hoc tests showed that females treated with ATD+F had a lower proportion of extra-pair offspring in their nests compared to controls (B = -1.74 ± 0.71, t24 = -2.43, P = 0.045, r = 0.44). Surprisingly, when compared to 127 control females, females treated with T also produced a lower proportion of extra-pair offspring (B = -1.98 ± 0.91, t24 = -2.16, P = 0.046, r = 0.40). There was no difference in the proportion of extra-pair offspring between nests where females were treated with ATD+F and T (B = 0.24 ± 0.90, t24 = 0.27, P = 0.79, r = 0.06). In the above analysis, I considered all nests with available paternity data; however, paternity data were incomplete for five nests due to the disappearance or breakage of a single egg in each of the nests. Performing the analysis with the exclusion of these data did not alter the conclusion. Clutch initiation date differed by treatment (F2, 28 = 3.52, P = 0.04). Post-hoc tests showed that T-treated females tended to initiate clutches later than control females (B = -7.90 ± 3.32, t28 = -2.38, P = 0.07, r = 0.44), but not ATD+F females (B = -5.25 ± 3.61, t28 = -1.46, P = 0.16, r = 0.27), and no difference in clutch initiation date was observed between ATD+F-treated and control females (B = -2.63 ± 1.83, t28 = -1.44, P = 0.16, r = 0.26). However, the trend for later breeding in the T treatment appeared to be due to the influence of three females that initiated their clutch approximately 25-34 days later than the other females in this treatment group. Removing these three females from the analysis showed that clutch initiation date no longer differed by treatment (F2, 25=1.41, P = 0.26). After controlling for clutch initiation date (B = -1.90 ± 0.75, F1, 28 = 6.45, P = 0.02), clutch sizes differed among treatment groups (F2, 28 = 3.31, P = 0.05). Post-hoc tests suggested that females treated with ATD+F tended to lay larger clutches than females in the T (B = 0.80 ± 0.39, t28 = 2.11, P = 0.065, r = 0.37) and control treatments (B = 0.96 ± 0.43, t28 = 2.20, P = 0.065, r = 0.38). Clutch size of control and T-treated females did not differ by treatment (B = 0.16 ± 0.44, t28 = -0.35, P = 0.73, r = -0.06). There also was a significant interaction between treatment and initiation date (F2, 28 = 3.58, P = 0.04, Figure 6.3). 128 Analyzing data by treatment revealed a negative relationship between clutch size and initiation date in control (B = -1.87 ± 0.73, F1, 9 = 6.61, P = 0.03, r = -0.63) and T treatment groups (B = -1.09 ± 0.34, F1, 9 = 9.92, P = 0.01, r = -0.72), but no relationship was detected in the ATD+F treatment group (B = 0.06 ± 0.26, F1, 10 = 0.05, P = 0.82, r = 0.07). As noted above, three T-treated females initiated their clutches later than the other T-treated females. Removal of these females from the analysis changed the results so that no effect of treatment on clutch size was detected (F2, 25 = 1.21, P = 0.31), even after controlling for clutch initiation date (B = -1.18 ± 0.43, F1, 25 = 7.70, P = 0.01); however, there was still a significant interaction between treatment and clutch initiation date (F2, 25 = 3.42, P = 0.048). This interaction was due to the decline in clutch size with later laying dates in control nests only (see above) as clutch sizes no longer declined with later clutch initiation dates in the T treatment once the three late-breeding birds were removed from the analysis (B = -0.24 ± 0.35, F1, 6 = 0.50, P = 0.51). Average egg mass did not differ among treatment groups (F2, 30 = 0.67, P = 0.52). After controlling for brood size (B = -0.63 ± 0.18, F1, 13 = 3.73, P = 0.08), nestlings were lighter at day 16 in the ATD+F than control treatments (B = -0.52 ± 0.29, F1, 13 = 6.05, P = 0.02, Figure 6.4). There also was a significant interaction between treatment and brood size (B = 0.80 ± 0.27, F1, 13 = 8.71, P = 0.01). Analyzing data by treatment indicated that there was a negative relationship between brood size and mass at day 16 for nestlings reared in control broods (B = -0.62 ± 0.15, F1, 5 = 16.12, P = 0.01), but no relationship was observed in ATD+F broods (B = 0.18 ± 0.18, F1, 8 = 1.00, P = 0.35). Nestlings reared in ATD+F and control broods did not differ in length of ninth primary (F1, 14 = 0.14, P = 0.72), even after controlling for time of measurement (F1, 14 = 3.89, P = 0.07), or length of head-bill 129 (F1, 15 = 2.02, P = 0.18). Nestlings grew at similar rates regardless of the treatment of the female (ninth primary, head-bill, and mass, all P-values > 0.16). Fledging success of nestlings did not differ by treatment (F1, 18 = 0.90, P = 0.35); 86.2 ± 0.06 % of nestlings in the ATD+F and 94.0 ± 0.04 % of nestling in the control treatments fledged successfully. 6.5. Discussion My aim was to examine whether T and its estrogen metabolites mediate behavioural traits that influence a female’s propensity to engage in extra-pair copulations. I predicted that if extra-pair copulation behaviour was correlated to the expression of aggressive or sexual behaviour, then altering hormones that mediate behavioural traits of females would also influence their extra-pair copulation behaviour. Females in the T treatment had elevated levels of androgens after being treated with T (Figure 6.1a), and produced fewer extra-pair offspring than control females (Figure 6.2). Although this result is contrary to my predictions, it demonstrates that altering T of female tree swallows influenced extra-pair copulation behaviour. In contrast, females treated with ATD+F did not have lower E2 concentrations than control females (Figure 6.1b); however, I suspect that this is due to collecting post-implant blood samples late in the season when females were provisioning nestlings. E2 concentrations decline seasonally in wild female passerines (e.g., Schwabl et al., 2014; Figure 6.1b), and I sampled females when E2 levels would have been naturally low in control females. While I recognize that a second blood sample collected at egg laying was needed to confirm differences in E2 between control and ATD+F females, this was not feasible as it almost certainly would have resulted in females abandoning their nesting attempt (e.g., Veiga et al., 2004). Nevertheless, that females treated with ATD+F had a lower 130 proportion of extra-pair offspring in their broods when compared to control females (Figure 6.2) demonstrates that the ATD+F treatment successfully altered the behaviour of females. Previous studies in a variety of taxa have reported that consistent individual differences in behavioural traits, or personalities, of females are related to variation in their extra-pair copulation behaviour. For example, female lizards that are more aggressive have a greater number of offspring in their litter sired by extra-pair males (While et al. 2009). Similarly, exploratory behaviour increased the probability of extra-pair offspring in the nests of female great tits (Parus major), but only when mated to a male with a similar personality type (e.g., fast-fast explorers; van Oers et al. 2008); however, a relationship between exploratory behaviour and extra-pair paternity was not detected in females from a different population of great tits (Patrick et al. 2012). Moreover, female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) that exhibited greater readiness to copulate with a male during their first sexual encounter in life were more likely to engage in extra-pair copulations once socially paired than females that did not copulate during their first encounter with a male (Forstmeier 2007). The low proportion of extra-pair offspring in ATD+F-treated, and presumably less aggressive or sexually receptive, females compared to control females in my study provides support for the hypothesis that personality may underlie variation in the rate of extra-pair paternity among females; however, the similarly low proportion of extra-pair paternity displayed by T-treated females suggests that increased aggressive behaviour in female birds does not appear to be associated with enhanced extra-pair copulation behaviour. Although I was unable to directly observe how my experimental manipulation altered the extra-pair copulation behaviour of females, due to the rarity of witnessing such behaviour in wild birds (e.g., Venier et al. 1993), I propose two mechanisms to explain the lower 131 proportion of extra-pair offspring in females treated with ATD+F (Figure 6.2). First, extrapair paternity in tree swallows often occurs when females visit neighbouring territories of other males to copulate (Venier et al. 1993), and being aggressive would facilitate a female’s ability to invade these territories that are defended by resident females (see Kempenaers et al., 1992). Therefore, by blocking the actions of T and its estrogenic metabolites to reduce aggressive behaviour of females in the ATD+F treatment, these females may have experienced limited opportunities for encountering extra-pair males in territories other than their own. Females treated with T, however, had similarly low levels of extra-pair paternity, which suggests that aggressive behaviour does not facilitate the pursuit of extra-pair copulations, although it cannot be ruled out that T-treated females similarly encountered fewer potential extra-pair mates if they spent more time involved in territory defence or were aggressive to potential extra-pair mates (see below). Second, the sexual behaviour of females may have been lowered in ATD+F treatment compared to controls as a result of blocking the estrogenic and androgenic actions of T. For example, T can mediate behaviour by being aromatized to E2 in the brain and acting on estrogen receptors (reviewed in Ball and Balthazart, 2008). In females, E2 mediates receptivity and solicitation behaviour (reviewed in Riters and Alger, 2011), and experimental studies in birds and mammals have demonstrated that these behaviours can be delayed or suppressed using an aromatase inhibitor (Rissman et al., 1990; Leboucher et al., 1998; Belle et al., 2005; but see Tomaszycki et al., 2006). While the lower proportion of extra-pair offspring in the nests of ATD+F females may be due to lower receptivity or solicitation behaviour as a result of ATD suppressing E2 concentrations, previous studies in male song sparrows (Melospiza melodia morphna) and both sexes of zebra finch reported ATD to be 132 ineffective at suppressing E2 production (Soma et al., 1999; Kabelik et al., 2010, respectively) and my blood samples collected during the nestling period cannot confirm that E2 concentrations in the ATD+F females were lower than control females during egg laying. As such, it is also possible that the lower proportion of extra-pair offspring in the nests of ATD+F females was due to F preventing T from binding directly to the androgen receptors. For example, female goats (Capra aegagrus hircus) treated with F are less receptive to males than control females (Imwalle and Katz, 2004); however, a previous study in blue tits reported that treating females with F alone did not influence extra-pair copulation behaviour as no difference in extra-pair paternity was observed between control and F-treated females (de Jong, 2013). Although my study is unable to determine whether the effect of the ATD+F on extra-pair copulation behaviour of female tree swallows is due to inhibiting aromatase activity, androgen receptor binding, or both, future studies could use implants containing E2 and other aromatase inhibitors, such as fadrozole, to examine the proximate effects of estrogenic actions of T on the mating behaviour of females. Contrary to my predictions, but consistent with previous studies in European starlings and blue tits (García-Vigón et al., 2008; de Jong, 2013), T-treated female tree swallows also had lower levels of extra-pair paternity than control females (Figure 6.5). García-Vigón et al. (2008) hypothesized that T-treated females may have produced fewer extra-pair offspring because T reduced the attractiveness of females, T-treated females invested more time in aggressive encounters and territory defence, or rejected copulations from extra-pair males because they were more aggressive. T has been shown to have masculinizing effects on female traits in a variety of taxa, although whether such changes reduce their attractiveness to potential mates is unclear (Ketterson et al., 2005; Lahaye et al., 2013). Given that 133 exogenous T increases aggressive behaviour in female tree swallows (Rosvall, 2013), in my study T-treated female tree swallows likely spent more time involved in aggressive interactions and territory defence, as previously suggested for spotless starlings (GarcíaVigón et al., 2008), rather than seeking extra-pair copulations. Implanting females with T prior to laying appeared to have negative consequences for reproductive success since females implanted with T during pre-laying did not incubate their eggs, even though they developed brood patches and remained active at their nest sites during the incubation and nestling periods of the breeding season (Berzins, personal observation). These results are similar to a previous study in tree swallows that experimentally elevated T levels of females during incubation and reported cooler nest temperatures and complete hatching failure (Rosvall, 2013). Female great tits implanted with T during nest building also had lower incubation temperatures and hatching success (de Jong et al., 2016), but such negative effects of T on incubation behaviour were not observed in female dark-eyed juncos implanted prior to breeding (Clotfelter et al., 2004). The results of my study and others suggest that T levels elevated after egg laying disrupt incubation behaviour in at least some species. In contrast, ATD+F females did not differ from control females in the initiation date of their clutches, clutch size or average egg mass. While female zebra finches treated with ATD+F produced fewer eggs compared to control females (Tomaszycki et al. 2006), I did not detect such effects of treating my birds with ATD+F, which may be due to using a lower dose of ATD+F in my study. Females treated with ATD+F did have lighter nestlings in their brood on day 16 compared to control females (Figure 6.4). Although I have no data on parental feeding rates, average mass of eggs (see Results), as well as mass of nestlings on day four did not differ between the ATD+F and 134 control treatments (P = 0.94; Berzins, unpublished data), suggesting the lower mass of nestlings on day 16 may be due to ATD+F-treated females or their mates provisioning nestlings less than in the control group. Nonetheless, despite nestlings reared in the ATD+F treatment being lighter on day 16, nestlings were of similar structural size, grew at similar rates, and were as likely to fledge as those raised by control females. In conclusion, my study demonstrates that experimentally altering exposure to T and its estrogenic metabolites prior to breeding in female tree swallows reduces the rate of extrapaternity in their broods. Although my results for ATD+F females were consistent with my predictions, the finding that T-treated females similarly produced a lower proportion of extra-pair offspring, although consistent with previous studies, was contrary to my predictions and suggests that elevated T does not enhance extra-pair mating of females. If Ttreated females were less likely to seek extra-pair copulations as a result of being more aggressive, then such behaviour may actually lower female fitness if females are seeking extra-pair copulations to improve offspring quality (e.g., see O’Brien and Dawson, 2007; Stapleton et al., 2007; Whittingham and Dunn, 2010). Overall my study suggests that extrapair paternity in tree swallows is driven partly by the behaviour of females. 135 Table 6.1. Summary of the number of female tree swallows that received implants containing testosterone (T) or 1,4,6-androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F), or empty implants (control). ‘Initiated a clutch’ refers to the number of females that laid at least one egg. ‘Hatched eggs’ refers to the number of females that hatched at least one egg. ‘Disappeared’ refers to females who were usurped or abandoned their nesting attempt, or whose eggs were depredated. Treatment Manipulated Initiated a clutch Hatched eggs Disappeared ATD+F Control 15 13 12 11 11 9 5 4 T 12 12 0 0 136 Figure 6.1. Concentrations of a) androgens and b) 17β-estradiol in female tree swallows measured prior to receiving implants during pre-breeding in May and after receiving implants containing testosterone or sham implants during the nestling period in June. Black circles represent control females, white circles represent testosterone (T)-treated females, and grey circles represent females treated with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F). Repeated measures for individuals are connected by a line, whereas individual circles indicate single measurements where repeated measurements were not available. Data points were jittered for easier interpretation. 137 Figure 6.2. Mean (± SE) proportion of extra-pair offspring in nests of tree swallows where females were either implanted with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F) or testosterone (T), and for controls (C; sham implanted). Sample sizes are given above error bars. 138 Figure 6.3. Relationship between clutch size and initiation date (1 January = 1) for nests where female tree swallows were implanted with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (dotted line), testosterone (dashed line), or received sham implants (solid line). Data points were jittered for easier interpretation. 139 Figure 6.4. Mean (± SE) body mass at day 16 of nestling tree swallows raised in broods where females were treated with 1,4,6 androstatrien-3, 17-dione and flutamide (ATD+F) and sham implants. Sample sizes indicate the number of broods within each treatment and are given above error bars. 140 Chapter 7: Synthesis Ornamental and behavioural traits expressed by female animals may improve their ability to attract mates and outcompete conspecifics for access to limited breeding opportunities and resources (Clutton-Brock, 2009; Rosvall, 2011a; Tobias et al., 2012). Although studies have demonstrated that such traits function as signals of quality that are preferred by males (Griggio et al., 2005; Torres and Velando, 2005) and are important in agonistic interactions (Griggio et al., 2010; Midamegbe et al., 2011; Morales et al., 2014), less is known about how ornamentation and behaviour influence the mating success of freeliving female birds. For my research, I used an experimental approach that altered the plumage brightness or testosterone (T)-mediated behaviour of female tree swallows to examine three main objectives: (1) whether variation in female plumage brightness is a signal of attractiveness that influences male investment in reproduction, including parental care (Chapter 2), social and extra-pair mate choice (Chapter 4) and male mating strategies (Chapter 4); (2) whether plumage brightness is a signal assessed by conspecific females that influences nest box retention (Chapter 3) and whether females with bright plumage experience social costs, such as lower reproductive success (Chapter 3) or nestling quality (Chapter 5); and (3) whether variation in plumage brightness (Chapter 4) or T-mediated behaviours (Chapter 6) influenced a female’s opportunity to engage in extra-pair copulations. Ornamental traits of females that are preferred by males may influence their investment in reproduction (Edward, 2015). For instance, variation in the attractiveness of ornamentation displayed by females may influence how much parental care males invest in the current reproductive attempt (Burley, 1986; Sheldon, 2000). Previous studies have demonstrated that males adjust their investment in parental care according to the manipulated 141 attractiveness of female ornaments (Burley, 1988; Roulin, 1999; Pilastro et al., 2003; Mahr et al., 2012); however, male tree swallows did not adjust their parental care according to the plumage brightness of females (Figure 2.1a), which is in contrast to these previous studies. My results did show that male tree swallows tended to provision nestlings in experimental broods (i.e., reduced and enhanced treatments) at a higher rate compared to control broods (Figure 2.2b). Although I performed my experiment after the hatching of eggs, as done in previous studies (Roulin, 1999; Pilastro et al., 2003; Mahr et al., 2012), my results show that male tree swallows only increased their provisioning rates in response to the sudden change in their mate’s ornamentation. This suggests that male tree swallows perceive the plumage brightness of females, but do not respond as would be predicted if such a trait was attractive to them (i.e., high provisioning of nestlings only for females with enhanced plumage brightness). In addition to differential allocation, male preference for ornamental traits of females can be investigated by examining time spent associating with or the courtship and copulation frequency directed towards females that differ in their attractiveness (Edward, 2015), as demonstrated by previous studies that have manipulated the ornamentation of females (Pilastro et al., 2003; Torres and Velando, 2005). For example, female rock sparrows (Petronia petronia) manipulated to display a breast patch reduced in size received less courtship from potential mates and they were also less likely to pair with a social mate (Griggio et al., 2005), although it is unclear whether less attractive female rock sparrows also paired with lower quality social mates. My results for Chapter 4 showed that the quality of social mate paired to females did not differ by plumage brightness treatment (Table 4.1), and females that switched nest sites, and presumably social mates, also did not pair with mates of 142 lower quality. Moreover, most females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment bred following the manipulation (Chapter 3), which is in contrast to the lower pairing success for less attractive female rock sparrows (Griggio et al., 2005). It is possible, however, that male tree swallows were constrained in their choice of a social mate since I performed the plumage manipulation after pair formation, and divorce may be a strategy that is costly (Choudhury, 1995). Nevertheless, if plumage brightness of female tree swallows is a trait that is attractive to males, then I predicted that males would alter their own mating strategies in relation to the plumage brightness treatment of the female, but I observed no evidence to support this (Table 4.2). Females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment mated with extra-pair males that had longer flight feathers (Figure 4.1), but given that plumage brightness does not appear to be a signal of attractiveness to males (Chapters 2, 4), this result may instead be due to social feedback from conspecifics (see below). Overall, the results I presented in Chapters 2 and 4 showing that investment in parental care, social mate choice, and mating strategies by males were not influenced by the plumage brightness treatment of the female indicate that positive assortative mating for plumage brightness in tree swallows is unlikely to be due to male mate choice for bright plumage displayed by females. Ornamental traits of females that are not preferred by males sometimes function as weaponry or signals of competitive ability or status during competition with conspecific females (e.g., Murphy et al., 2009b; Watson and Simmons, 2010a, b). Even in humans (Homo sapiens), studies have demonstrated that females purchase luxury items to signal high quality when competing against conspecific females for mates (Hudders et al., 2014). In tree swallows, competition among females for access to, and in defence of, males with a nest site is intense and can lead to injury or death (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985; Rosvall, 2008), so 143 it is perhaps not surprising that plumage brightness of female tree swallows is a signal assessed by conspecific females (Chapters 2, 3). Since tree swallows show positive assortative mating for plumage brightness (Bitton et al., 2008), and my results provide no evidence of male mate choice as the underlying mechanism (Chapters 2, 4), assortative mating in tree swallows likely results from females with bright plumage having greater competitive ability and outcompeting duller conspecific females for males with a nest site. A previous study in tree swallows reported that the dull brown plumage of second-year (SY) females is a signal of low competitive ability (Coady and Dawson, 2013), and so positive assortative mating occurring as a result of females with bright plumage having greater competitive ability is in line with this previous study. While my results provide evidence that bright plumage of female tree swallows functions in agonistic interactions with conspecific females, I was unable to directly observe interactions among females to confirm such a signalling role. Nevertheless, the results of Chapters 2 and 3 are consistent with plumage brightness of female tree swallows functioning as a status signal. Status signals that honestly reflect quality should be used by individuals to assess the competitive ability of conspecifics so that costly agonistic interactions over contested resources can be avoided (reviewed in Senar, 2006); however, whether signals that reflect competitive ability are trusted or tested may depend on the value of the resource compared to the cost of fighting (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1988; Tibbetts, 2008). The results of Chapter 2 are consistent with signals of competitive ability being trusted since I only observed evidence to suggest that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment experienced social interactions with conspecifics. For instance, females whose plumage brightness was manipulated after the hatching of eggs likely appeared as strangers to 144 neighbouring females, but only females with reduced plumage brightness signalling low competitive ability may have been challenged by neighbouring females, leading to lower offspring quality and fledging success (Figure 2.4). In contrast, the results of the nest-site retention experiment presented in Chapter 3 show that the competitive ability of females in all plumage brightness treatments were tested (Table 3.2). Signals of quality may be tested during the pre-breeding period because males with a nest site are a valuable resource since they are limited (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985) and without a male and nest site, female tree swallows are unable to breed. Murphy et al. (2009a) similarly reported that female streak-backed orioles (Icterus pustulatus pustulatus) responded with greater intensity to model intruders that were manipulated to signal high competitive ability instead of trusting status signals and relinquishing a valuable resource. Overall, my results and the results of previous studies (e.g., Murphy et al., 2009a) show that conspecifics may challenge females regardless of the competitive ability signalled by their plumage when in possession of a valuable resource, such as a nest site. Although the results I report in Chapters 2 and 4 are consistent with a status signalling function of bright plumage displayed by female tree swallows, testing this directly would prove difficult in an aerial insectivore species that cannot easily be moved into aviaries to conduct dominance trials for access to resources where signals of status should be trusted (e.g., Murphy et al., 2009b). While manipulating the plumage brightness of model intruders and simulating territory intrusions would allow me to directly observe how conspecifics respond to females with enhanced plumage brightness, and corroborate my results for Chapter 3, such an experiment would only demonstrate a role for female plumage brightness to function in agonistic interactions and not necessarily confirm status signalling (Amundsen, 2000a). 145 Ornamental traits that function as signals of attractiveness to potential mates or competitive ability to conspecifics should honestly reflect the underlying quality of their bearer; however, few studies have examined whether the honesty of ornaments displayed by free-living female birds is maintained by social enforcement. In Chapter 3, the results of the nest-site retention experiment demonstrate that nest-site intrusions from conspecific females may provide a mechanism that enforces honest ornamentation; females that dishonestly signalled high quality were less able to retain their nest site than females displaying lowquality signals. Moreno et al. (2013) proposed that the honesty of ornamentation displayed by females may be enforced by social control after providing experimental evidence that non-ornamented female pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) manipulated to dishonestly display a forehead patch had higher levels of blood malondialdehydes, indicative of oxidative damage, compared to control females. My results providing support for the social control hypothesis (Chapter 3) are consistent with Moreno et al. (2013) and suggest that social control may maintain the honesty of ornamental traits in female birds. Social control of honest signalling should impose costs on dishonest signallers, such as lower reproductive success, increased energy expenditure (Kotiaho, 2001), or lower investment in parental care (Qvarnström, 1997). The results I present in Chapters 3 and 5 showing that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment dishonestly signalling high quality initiated their clutches later and produced nestlings of smaller structural size than females in the control and reduced plumage brightness treatments (Figure 3.1a; Figure 5.1) provide evidence of social costs imposed on females dishonestly signalling high quality. My results for females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment are consistent with previous studies in tree swallows reporting that more ornamented females produced low- 146 quality nestlings (Coady, 2011; Bentz and Siefferman, 2013). Coady (2011) hypothesized that more ornamented females may experience costs associated with increased aggression from conspecific females challenging the status signalled by their plumage ornamentation. If female tree swallows with bright plumage engage in agonistic interactions with one another to enforce signal honesty, as my results for nest retention suggest (Chapter 3), then my results provide evidence of social costs experienced by more ornamented female tree swallows. Costs associated with possession of ornamental plumage also have been observed in other species. For example, non-ornamented female pied flycatchers manipulated to dishonestly signal a forehead patch had increased oxidative damage compared to control females, and the level of oxidative damage was similar to that observed in females naturally displaying a forehead patch. These results are in contrast to female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica erthrogaster) manipulated to signal darker ventral plumage (Vitousek et al., 2013), and may differ from tree swallows and pied flycatchers if signals of quality are trusted by conspecific females (see Vitousek et al., 2016). Interestingly, female tree swallows in the reduced plumage brightness treatment dishonestly signalling low quality did not appear to suffer social costs since they were more likely to retain their nest sites and breed (Table 3.2) and did not differ from control females for any measure of reproductive success or nestling quality (Chapter 3, 5). While it is possible that females dishonestly signalling low quality may pay a cost by acquiring a low-quality social (Bitton et al., 2008) or extra-pair mate, females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment were not paired to lower quality social mates (Table 4.2), although this is could be because they had already acquired mates when I manipulated their plumage brightness, and they did not mate with low-quality extra-pair males (Table 4.4). Without a cost imposed on dishonest signallers, the signalling system 147 would be susceptible to dishonesty (e.g., Owens and Hartley, 1991); therefore, it is possible that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment incurred costs that I did not measure, such as physiological costs, or were involved in fewer agonistic interactions overall with conspecifics if they were perceived as a low competitive threat to more ornamented females (Coady and Dawson, 2013). Disadvantages of dishonestly signalling low quality for female tree swallows also may occur during the non-breeding season, such as on wintering roost sites, but this explanation requires further investigation. Clearly, further studies that manipulate the ornamental traits of female birds so that they signal higher and lower quality prior to breeding and during the non-breeding season, followed by the observation of agonistic interactions among conspecifics, would be valuable. Recently, several empirical studies and reviews have highlighted the importance of social feedback from potential mates and conspecifics about ornament quality on the physiology or behaviour of signallers (e.g., Safran et al., 2008; Rubenstein and Hauber, 2008; Vitousek et al., 2013; Dey et al., 2014; Vitousek et al., 2014b). Results from two of my chapters add to this literature by showing that social feedback from conspecifics may alter the behaviour or physiology of females with manipulated plumage brightness. In Chapter 2, lower nestling quality and fledging success for broods reared by females in the reduced plumage brightness and enlarged brood size treatment (Figure 2.4) suggest that females in the reduced plumage brightness treatment may have experienced social interactions with conspecifics that were stressful, which may have elevated levels of the stress hormone corticosterone, and when combined with the demands of rearing an enlarged brood size may have caused females to abandon their nesting attempt. Although I have no behavioural or hormonal data to support such a mechanism, female barn swallows with elevated levels of 148 corticosterone prior to incubation were more likely to abandon their nesting attempt (Vitousek et al., 2014a) and experimentally elevating corticosterone of female tree swallows resulted in lower nestling survival (Ouyang et al., 2015). In Chapter 4, females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment mated with extra-pair mates that were of higher quality than their social mates (Figure 4.1). Since females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment may have been challenged by naturally bright, high-quality females testing the quality of their ornamentation (Chapter 3), those females that defeated conspecifics in agonistic interactions and retained their nest site could have perceived themselves as strong competitors and pursued extra-pair copulations from high-quality males on the territories of other females. Such ‘winner effects’ on the behaviour of female tree swallows may be facilitated by changes in physiology, such as increased androgen levels (Oliveira et al., 2009). If females with experimentally enhanced plumage brightness had higher T levels as a result of defeating conspecifics, this would provide further support that these females experienced social costs, and suggest a mechanism for why they bred later (Figure 3.1a) and produced low-quality nestlings (Figure 5.1) compared to females in the reduced and control plumage brightness treatments. For example, experimental elevation of T in females birds delays egg laying (Chapter 6; Clotfelter et al., 2004, Veiga and Polo, 2008) and increases aggressive behaviour (Rosvall, 2013), and females that are more aggressive provision their nestlings less and produce low-quality nestlings (Rosvall, 2011). This mechanism is consistent with Qvarnström (1997), who reported that yearling male collared flycatchers manipulated to display an enlarged forehead patch spent more time competing with conspecifics and provisioned their nestlings less (i.e., ornament-parental care trade-off) compared to control males. 149 Although the results I present in Chapter 4 are consistent with females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment that defeated conspecifics in agonistic interactions having elevated levels of T that facilitated the pursuit of extra-pair copulations from highquality extra-pair mates (see above; Figure 4.1), the proportion of extra-pair offspring in the broods of females with enhanced plumage brightness did not differ from controls (Table 4.3). Such a result could indicate that levels of T in female tree swallows are not related to extrapair paternity, consistent with a previous study of dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) that reported no difference in the level of extra-pair paternity in broods of T-treated and control females (Gerlach and Ketterson, 2013), but inconsistent with the results I report for female tree swallows treated with T (Chapter 6). A lower proportion of extra-pair offspring observed in the broods of T-treated female tree swallows (Figure 6.2) may differ from the results I present for females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment in Chapter 4 for two reasons. First, if females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment experienced winner effects after repeated challenges from conspecifics, and their signal and behaviour became congruent (Vitousek et al., 2014), then they may have been perceived as high quality and challenged less by conspecifics, resulting in lower levels of T. For example, levels of T decreased within one week in female barn swallows manipulated to display darker ventral plumage (Vitousek et al., 2013), and this may be due to high-quality signals of female barn swallows being trusted (Vitousek et al., 2016). This mechanism seems unlikely, however, as signals of quality should be tested when the value of the resource (i.e., breeding opportunity) is high (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1988; Tibbetts, 2008) and female tree swallows are nest-site limited (Leffelaar and Robertson, 1985). Because I was interested in examining extra-pair paternity, I was unable to capture 150 females following the plumage brightness manipulation and collect blood samples to examine whether androgen levels decreased or increased, as it would have likely resulted in females abandoning their nesting attempt (e.g., Veiga et al., 2004). Second, although levels of androgens in T-treated females appear to be at the upper limit of pre-breeding levels of T (Figure 6.2.), it is possible that the dose of T used in my study (Chapter 6) was too high and increased aggressive behaviour (Rosvall, 2013) beyond levels that would occur naturally in female tree swallows. As a consequence, T-treated females may have spent most of their time involved in agonistic interactions instead of pursuing extra-pair copulations (García-Vigón et al., 2008). Nevertheless, whether females displaying enhanced plumage brightness had altered levels of T as a result of a social feedback mechanisms (Vitousek et al., 2014; Chapter 4) or T was elevated experimentally (Chapter 6), my results suggest that T does not enhance extra-pair paternity in tree swallows. In Chapter 6, female tree swallows treated with 1,4,6-androstatrien-3, 17-dione (ATD) and flutamide (F) in combination (ATD+F) to block the estrogenic and androgenic actions of T produced fewer extra-pair offspring compared to controls (Figure 6.2). A previous study in blue tits that treated females only with F, reported no difference in the level of extra-pair paternity between F-treated females and controls (de Jong, 2013). Consequently, the lower proportion of extra-pair paternity in the broods of females treated with ATD+F in my study may be due to ATD blocking the conversion of T to 17β-estradiol (E2). Blocking the effects of E2 may have lowered the receptivity or solicitation behaviour of females (Rissman et al., 1990; Leboucher et al., 1998; Belle et al., 2005; but see Tomaszycki et al., 2006), leading to the production of fewer extra-pair offspring, although it is important to note that my blood samples collected late in the breeding season (June) cannot confirm 151 that E2 was lower in ATD+F treated females during egg laying (Figure 6.2). Nevertheless, studies in other taxa, such as goats (Capra aegagrus hircus), also have demonstrated that receptivity is lower in females treated with F (Imwalle and Katz, 2004). Therefore, blocking the actions of both T and E2 may influence the receptivity or solicitation behaviour of female tree swallows, and suggest that extra-pair paternity is related to sexual behaviour of females (Chapter 6). Forstmeier (2007) also reported that extra-pair paternity in female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) was related to sexual behaviour; females with extra-pair offspring in their brood were more likely to have copulated with a male during their first sexual encounter in life (Forstmeier, 2007). Although my results show that sexual and extra-pair copulation behaviour may be mediated by the estrogenic and/or androgenic actions of T, it is possible that such a relationship exists because they are part of a larger behavioural syndrome. For instance, sexual behaviour, such as receptivity, is part of a behavioural syndrome associated with the expression of melanin-based plumage and so females with darker plumage may have greater sexual receptivity towards males (Ducrest et al., 2008). Indeed, a recent study in female yellow warblers (Setophaga petechia) proposed that higher levels of extra-pair paternity in broods of females displaying greater melanin-based, but less carotenoid-based plumage, may be explained by such females pairing with low-quality mates or being mate guarded less intensively because of their less ornamented dull carotenoid plumage, but also having a greater propensity to seek or engage in extra-pair copulations due to the high coverage of melanin-based plumage (Grunst and Grunst, 2014). Although female tree swallows have structurally-based plumage, and experimentally altered plumage brightness is not associated with the rate of extra-pair paternity in the broods of females (Table 4.3), the high levels of extra-pair paternity in this species may be due to females with 152 different personalities having different mating strategies. Future studies should explore whether females with personality traits that are more bold, exploratory, and receptive towards males have a greater proportion of extra-pair paternity in their brood, and whether such traits are also indicative of whether females engage in extra-pair copulations with neighbouring males or with males from long distances away from the breeding colony at feeding and roosting sites. If female receptivity underlies extra-pair copulation behaviour of female tree swallows, it also may explain why offspring in the broods of some females are sired by up to 4 males (e.g., Whittingham and Dunn, 2014). The function of ornaments and behaviours expressed by female birds was historically neglected by scientists (Amundsen, 2000a), and although the number of empirical studies investigating how the traits of females influence their ability to acquire mates and compete against conspecifics has increased in recent decades (reviews in Clutton-Brock, 2009; Rosvall, 2011a; Tobias et al., 2012), our understanding of how such traits influence the mating success of free-living wild female birds is limited. Moreover, in species that are similarly ornamented, males may not prefer females displaying elaborate ornaments (Chapters 2, 4) and so more studies are needed to examine how the ornamentation of females functions as signals of competitive ability or status in competition among conspecifics (reviewed in Tarvin and Murphy, 2012). My research in female tree swallows shows that bright plumage functions in agonistic interactions because the honesty of bright plumage appears to be socially enforced by conspecific females (Chapter 3), and females that dishonestly signal high quality suffer social costs (Chapters 3, 5). Few studies have examined whether the honesty of elaborate ornaments is maintained by social enforcement in freeliving female birds, but my results and those of Moreno et al. (2013) suggest that honesty 153 may be maintained by social control. This finding highlights the importance of manipulating the ornamental traits of females not just during the nestling provisioning period, but also prior to breeding. Extra-pair paternity is a phenomenon that has intrigued scientists for decades, but no previous study that I am aware of has manipulated the ornamentation of females to examine whether this influences a female’s opportunity to engage in extra-pair copulations or female choice of extra-pair mates. Several studies have been unable to detect differences in phenotypic traits between a female’s social and extra-pair mate (reviewed in Hsu et al., 2015). My results showing that females in the enhanced plumage brightness treatment mated with extra-pair mates that had longer flight feathers than their social mate (Figure 4.1) suggest that social feedback regarding signal quality from conspecifics may influence female choice of or ability to pursue extra-pair mates. Additionally, few studies have manipulated the behaviour of females by altering their exposure to T to examine extrapair paternity (see García-Vigón et al., 2008; Gerlach and Ketterson, 2013; de Jong, 2013 for examples). My research also highlights the importance of examining the estrogenic actions of T on extra-pair copulation behaviour. Moreover, further studies that examine how the receptivity of female birds influence extra-pair paternity and whether extra-pair paternity is related to personality traits as part of a behavioural syndrome would prove valuable. 154 8.1. References Albrecht T, Schnitzer J, Kreisinger J, Exnerová A, Bryja J & Munclinger P (2007) Extrapair paternity and the opportunity for sexual selection in long-distant migratory passerines. Behavioral Ecology 18: 477–486. Albrecht T, Vinkler M, Schnitzer J, Poláková R, Munclinger P & Bryja J (2009) Extra-pair fertilizations contribute to selection on secondary male ornamentation in a socially monogamous passerine. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22: 2020–2030. Amundsen T (2000a) Female ornaments: genetically correlated or sexually selected? In Espmark, Y., Amundsen, T., Rosenqvist, G. (editors) Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication. Tapir Academic Press, Trondheim, pp. 133–154. Amundsen T (2000b) Why are female birds ornamented? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15: 149–155. Amundsen T, Forsgren E & Hansen LTT (1997) On the function of female ornaments: male bluethroats prefer colourful females. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 264: 1579–1586. Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. Archawaranon M & Wiley, RH (1988) Control of aggression and dominance in whitethroated sparrows by testosterone and its metabolites. Hormones and Behavior 22: 497– 517. Ardia DR (2007) Site- and sex-level differences in adult feeding behaviour and its consequences to offspring quality in tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) following broodsize manipulation. Canadian Journal of Zoology 85: 847–854 Ardia DR & Clotfelter ED (2007) Individual quality and age affect responses to an energetic constraint in a cavity-nesting bird. Behavioral Ecology 18: 259–266. Balenger SL, Johnson LS & Masters BS (2009) Sexual selection in a socially monogamous bird: male color predicts paternity success in the mountain bluebird, Sialia currucoides. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63: 403–411. Ball GF & Balthazart J (2008) Individual variation and the endocrine regulation of behaviour and physiology in birds: a cellular/molecular perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 363: 1699–1710. Ballentine B & Hill GE (2003) Female mate choice in relation to structural plumage coloration in blue grosbeaks. The Condor 105: 593–598. 155 Balthazart J, Schumacher M & Evrard L (1990) Sex differences and steroid control of testosterone-metabolizing enzyme activity in the quail brain. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 2: 675–683. Barber CJ & Robertson R (2007) Timing of copulations and the pattern of paternity in relation to laying order in tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor. Journal of Avian Biology 38: 249–254. Barnett CA, Thompson CF & Sakaluk SK (2012) Aggressiveness, boldness and parental food provisioning in male house wrens (Troglodytes aedon). Ethology 118: 984–993. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B & Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–49. Belle MDC, Sharp PJ & Lea RW (2005) Aromatase inhibition abolishes courtship behaviours in the ring dove (Streptopelia risoria) and reduces androgen and progesterone receptors in the hypothalamus and anterior pituitary gland. Molecular and Cellular Biochemistry 276: 193–204. Bentz AB & Siefferman L (2013) Age-dependent relationships between coloration and reproduction in a species exhibiting delayed plumage maturation in females. Journal of Avian Biology 44: 080–088. Berglund A, Bisazza A & Pilastro A (1996) Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 58: 385–399. Berzins LL & Dawson RD (2016) Experimentally altered plumage brightness of female tree swallows: a test of the differential allocation hypothesis. Behaviour153: 525–550. Birkhead TR & Møller AP (1992) Sperm competition in birds: evolutionary causes and consequences. Academic Press, London. Biro PA & Stamps JA (2008) Are animal personality traits linked to life-history productivity? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23: 361–368. Bitton P-P & Dawson RD (2008) Age-related differences in plumage characteristics of male tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor: hue and brightness signal different aspects of individual quality. Journal of Avian Biology 39: 446–452. Bitton P-P, Dawson RD & Ochs CL (2008) Plumage characteristics, reproductive investment and assortative mating in tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 1543–1550. Bitton P-P, O’Brien EL & Dawson RD (2007) Plumage brightness and age predict extrapair fertilization success of male tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor. Animal Behaviour 74: 1777–1784. 156 Bolour S & Braunstein G (2005) Testosterone therapy in women: a review. International Journal of Impotence Research 17: 399–408. Bortolotti LE, Harriman VB, Clark RG & Dawson RD (2011) Can changes in provisioning by parent birds account for seasonally declining patterns of offspring recruitment? Canadian Journal of Zoology 89: 921–928. Boyle WA, Winkler DW & Guglielmo CG (2012) Rapid loss of fat but not lean mass prior to chick provisioning supports the flight efficiency hypothesis in tree swallows. Functional Ecology 26: 895–903. Brown ME (1996) Assessing body condition in birds. In Nolan V Jr, Ketterson ED (editors) Current ornithology, Volume 13 Springer US, New York, pp.67-135. Browne WJ, Subramanian SV, Jones K & Goldstein H (2005) Variance partitioning in multilevel logistic models that exhibit overdispersion. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society) 168: 599–613. Burley N (1986) Sexual selection for aesthetic traits in species with biparental care. The American Naturalist 127: 415–445. Burley N (1988) The differential-allocation hypothesis: an experimental test. The American Naturalist 132: 611–628. Canoine V & Gwinner E (2002) Seasonal differences in the hormonal control of territorial aggression in free-living European stonechats. Hormones and Behavior 41: 1–8. Carere C, Drent PJ, Privitera L, Koolhaas JM & Groothuis TGG (2005) Personalities in great tits, Parus major: stability and consistency. Animal Behaviour 70: 795–805. Choudhury S (1995) Divorce in birds: a review of the hypotheses. Animal Behaviour 50: 413–429. Chuang-Dobbs HC, Webster MS, Holmes RT (2001) The effectiveness of mate guarding by male black-throated blue warblers. Behavioral Ecology 12: 541–546. Cleasby IR & Nakagawa S (2011) Neglected biological patterns in the residuals. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 2361–2372. Clotfelter ED, O’Neal DM, Gaudioso JM, Casto JM, Parker-Renga IM, Snajdr EA, Duffy DL, Nolan V Jr. & Ketterson ED (2004) Consequences of elevating plasma testosterone in females of a socially monogamous songbird: evidence of constraints on male evolution? Hormones and Behavior 46: 171–178. Clutton-Brock T (2009). Sexual selection in females. Animal Behaviour 77: 3–11. 157 Coady CD (2011) The signaling function of plumage characteristics in female tree swallows and implications for reproductive success and life-history decisions. MSc Thesis, University of Norther British Columbia, Canada. Coady CD & Dawson RD (2013) Subadult plumage color of female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) reduces conspecific aggression during the breeding season. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 125: 348–357. Cornwallis CK & Birkhead TR (2007) Experimental evidence that female ornamentation increases the acquisition of sperm and signals fecundity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 274: 583–590. Cornwallis CK & O’Connor EA (2009) Sperm: seminal fluid interactions and the adjustment of sperm quality in relation to female attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 276: 3467–3475. Cote J, Clobert J, Brodin T, Fogarty S & Sih A (2010) Personality-dependent dispersal: characterization, ontogeny and consequences for spatially structured populations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365: 4065–4076. Cotton AJ, Cotton S, Small J & Pomiankowski A (2015) Male mate preference for female eyespan and fecundity in the stalk-eyed fly, Teleopsis dalmanni. Behavioral Ecology 26: 376–385. Cotton S, Small J & Pomiankowski A (2006) Sexual selection and condition-dependent mate preferences. Current Biology 16: R755–R765. Crawley, M.J., 2013. The R book, 2 edition. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., West Sussex, United Kingdom. Creighton E (2001) Mate acquisition in the European blackbird and its implications for sexual strategies. Ethology Ecology and Evolution 13: 247-260. Cristol DA & Johnsen TS (1994) Spring arrival, aggression and testosterone in female redwinged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). The Auk 111: 210–214. Crowe SA, Kleven O, Delmore KE, Laskemoen T, Nocera JJ, Lifjeld JT & Robertson RJ (2009) Paternity assurance through frequent copulations in a wild passerine with intense sperm competition. Animal Behaviour 77: 183–187 Cuervo JJ, de Lope F & Møller AP (1996) The function of long tails in female barn swallows (Hirundo rustica ): an experimental study. Behavioral Ecology 7: 132–136. 158 Dakin R, Lendvai ÁZ, Ouyang JQ, Moore IT, & Bonier F (2016) Plumage colour is associated with partner parental care in mutually ornamented tree swallows. Animal Behaviour 111: 111–118. Dale S & Slagsvold T (1995) Female contests for nest sites and mates in the pied flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca. Ethology 99: 209–222. Darwin C (1871) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. David M, Auclair Y & Cézilly F (2011) Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding context. Animal Behaviour 81: 219–224. Dawson RD (2008) Timing of breeding and environmental factors as determinants of reproductive performance of tree swallows. Canadian Journal of Zoology 86: 843–850. Dawson RD & Bidwell MT (2005) Dietary calcium limits size and growth of nestling tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor in a non-acidified landscape. Journal of Avian Biology 36: 127–134. Dawson RD & Bortolotti GR (2003) Parental effort of American kestrels: the role of variation in brood size. Canadian Journal of Zoology 81: 852–860. Dawson RD, Lawrie CC & O’Brien EL (2005) The importance of microclimate variation in determining size, growth and survival of avian offspring: experimental evidence from a cavity nesting passerine. Oecologia 144: 499–507. de Jong B (2013) Testosterone, a female hormone: testing the function and evolution of testosterone in female birds. PhD dissertation, Ghent University, Belgium. de Jong B, Lens L, Amininasab SM, van Oers K, Darras VM, Eens M, Pinxten R, Komdeur J & Groothuis TGG (2016) Effects of experimentally sustained elevated testosterone on incubation behaviour and reproductive success in female great tits (Parus major). General and Comparative Endocrinology 230–231: 38–47. Dey CJ, Dale J & Quinn JS (2014) Manipulating the appearance of a badge of status causes changes in true badge expression. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 281: 20132680. Dingemanse NJ, Kazem AJN, Réale D & Wright J (2010) Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual plasticity. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 25: 81–89. Doutrelant C, Grégoire A, Grnac N, Gomez D, Lambrechts MM & Perret P (2008) Female coloration indicates female reproductive capacity in blue tits. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21: 226–233. 159 Doyle A & Siefferman L (2014) Supplemental food increases ornamentation of male nestling Eastern Bluebirds. Journal of Field Ornithology 85: 31–39. Ducrest A-L, Keller L & Roulin A (2008) Pleiotropy in the melanocortin system, coloration and behavioural syndromes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23: 502–510. Dunn P & Whittingham L (2005) Radio-tracking of female tree swallows prior to egg-laying. Journal of Field Ornithology 76: 259–263. Dunn PO, Lifjeld JT & Whittingham LA (2009) Multiple paternity and offspring quality in tree swallows. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63: 911–922. Edward DA (2015) The description of mate choice. Behavioral Ecology 26: 301–310. Edward DA & Chapman T (2011) The evolution and significance of male mate choice. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 26: 647–654. Eising CM, Eikenaar C, Schwabl H & Groothuis TGG (2001) Maternal androgens in blackheaded gull (Larus ridibundus) eggs: consequences for chick development. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 268: 839–846. Elias DO, Botero CA, Andrade MCB, Mason AC & Kasumovic MM (2010) High resource valuation fuels “desperado” fighting tactics in female jumping spiders. Behavioral Ecology 21: 868–875. Estep LK, Mays H Jr, Keyser AJ, Ballentine B & Hill GE (2005) Effects of breeding density and plumage coloration on mate guarding and cuckoldry in blue grosbeaks (Passerina caerulea). Canadian Journal of Zoology 83: 1143–1148. Ferns PN & Hinsley SA (2004) Immaculate tits: head plumage pattern as an indicator of quality in birds. Animal Behaviour 67: 261–272. Field A, Miles J & Field Z (2012) Discovering statistics using R. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, California. Fitzpatrick S, Berglund A & Rosenqvist G (1995) Ornaments or offspring: costs to reproductive success restrict sexual selection processes. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 55: 251–260. Forstmeier W (2007) Do individual females differ intrinsically in their propensity to engage in extra-pair copulations? PLoS ONE 2: e952. Forstmeier W, Nakagawa S, Griffith SC, & Kempenaers B (2014) Female extra-pair mating: adaptation or genetic constraint? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 29: 456–464. 160 Fox J & Weisberg S (2011) An R companion to applied regression, second edition. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, California. Gangestad SW, Thornhill R & Garver CE (2002) Changes in women’s sexual interests and their partner’s mate–retention tactics across the menstrual cycle: evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 269: 975–982. Gangestad SW, Thornhill R & Garver-Apgar CE (2005) Women’s sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle depend on primary partner developmental instability. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 272: 2023–2027. García-Navas V, Ferrer ES, Sanz JJ (2012) Plumage yellowness predicts foraging ability in the blue tit Cyanistes caeruleus. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 106: 418–429. García-Vigón E, Cordero PJ & Veiga JP (2008) Exogenous testosterone in female spotless starlings reduces their rate of extrapair offspring. Animal Behaviour 76: 345–353. Garson GD (2012) Hierarchical linear modeling: guide and applications. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, California Gerlach NM & Ketterson ED (2013) Experimental elevation of testosterone lowers fitness in female dark-eyed juncos. Hormones and Behavior 63: 782–790. Gladbach A, Gladbach DJ, Kempenaers B & Quillfeldt P (2010) Female-specific colouration, carotenoids and reproductive investment in a dichromatic species, the upland goose Chloephaga picta leucoptera. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 64: 1779–1789. Gowaty PA (2008) Reproductive compensation. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 21: 1189– 1200. Grafen A (1990) Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology 144: 517– 546. Griffith SC, Owens IPF & Thuman KA (2002) Extra pair paternity in birds: a review of interspecific variation and adaptive function. Molecular Ecology 11: 2195–2212. Griffiths R, Double MC, Orr K & Dawson RJG (1998) A DNA test to sex most birds. Molecular Ecology 7: 1071–1075. Griggio M, Devigili A, Hoi H & Pilastro A (2009) Female ornamentation and directional male mate preference in the rock sparrow. Behavioral Ecology 20: 1072–1078. Griggio M, Valera F, Casas A & Pilastro A (2005). Males prefer ornamented females: a field experiment of male choice in the rock sparrow. Animal Behaviour 69: 1243–1250. 161 Griggio M, Zanollo V & Hoi H (2010). Female ornamentation, parental quality, and competitive ability in the rock sparrow. Journal of Ethology 28: 455–462. Grunst AS & Grunst ML (2014) Multiple sexual pigments, assortative social pairing, and genetic paternity in the yellow warbler (Setophaga petechia). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 68: 1451–1463. Hainstock MH, Smith MC, Carr J & Shutler D (2010) Parental investment and brood value in tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor. Behaviour 147: 441–464. Hall ZJ & MacDougall-Shackleton SA (2012) Influence of testosterone metabolites on songcontrol system neuroplasticity during photostimulation in adult European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). PLoS ONE 7: e40060. Haller J, Fuchs E, Halász J & Makara GB (1999) Defeat is a major stressor in males while social instability is stressful mainly in females: towards the development of a social stress model in female rats. Brain Research Bulletin 50: 33–39. Harriman VB, Dawson RD, Clark RG, Fairhurst GD & Bortolotti GR (2013) Effects of ectoparasites on seasonal variation in quality of nestling tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Canadian Journal of Zoology 92: 87–96. Harris WE & Uller T (2009) Reproductive investment when mate quality varies: differential allocation versus reproductive compensation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 364: 1039–1048. Hasegawa M, Watanabe M & Nakamura M (2015) Promiscuous copulation attempts and discriminate pairing displays in male barn swallows as revealed by model presentation. Ethology Ecology and Evolution 28: 163–174. Hau M, Wikelski M, Soma KK & Wingfield JC (2000) Testosterone and year-round territorial aggression in a tropical bird. General and Comparative Endocrinology 117: 20– 33. Henderson LJ, Heidinger BJ, Evans NP & Arnold KE (2013) Ultraviolet crown coloration in female blue tits predicts reproductive success and baseline corticosterone. Behavioral Ecology 24: 1299–1305. Hervé M (2016) RVAideMemoire: diverse basic statistical and graphical functions. R package version 0.9-53. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package= RVAideMemoire. Hidalgo-Garcia S (2006) The carotenoid-based plumage coloration of adult blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus correlates with the health status of their brood. Ibis 148: 727–734. Hinde CA (2006) Negotiation over offspring care?—a positive response to partnerprovisioning rate in great tits. Behavioral Ecology 17: 6–12. 162 Hinde CA & Kilner RM (2007) Negotiations within the family over the supply of parental care. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 274: 53–60. Holveck M-J & Riebel K (2009) Low-quality females prefer low-quality males when choosing a mate. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 277: 153–160. Horváthová T, Nakagawa S & Uller T (2012) Strategic female reproductive investment in response to male attractiveness in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 279: 163–170. Houtman AM & Falls JB (1994) Negative assortative mating in the white-throated sparrow, Zonotrichia albicollis: the role of mate choice and intra-sexual competition. Animal Behaviour 48: 377–383. Hsu Y, Earley RL & Wolf LL (2006) Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biological Reviews 81: 33–74. Hsu Y-H, Schroeder J, Winney I, Burke T & Nakagawa S (2015) Are extra-pair males different from cuckolded males? A case study and a meta-analytic examination. Molecular Ecology 24: 1558–1571. Hudders L, Backer CD, Fisher M & Vyncke P (2014) The rival wears prada: luxury consumption as a female competition strategy. Evolutionary Psychology 12: 570–587. Huhta E, Rytkönen S & Solonen T (2003) Plumage brightness of prey increases predation risk: an among-species comparison. Ecology 84: 1793–1799. Hussell DJ (1983) Age and plumage color in female tree swallows. Journal of Field Ornithology 54: 312–318. IBM Corp., 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. IBM Corp., Armonk, NY. Imwalle DB & Katz LS (2004) Divergent roles for estrogens and androgens in the expression of female goat sexual behavior. Hormones and Behavior 46: 54–58. Jacobs AC, Fair JM & Zuk M (2015) Coloration, paternity, and assortative mating in western bluebirds. Ethology 121: 176–186. Jawor JM, Gray N, Beall SM & Breitwisch R (2004) Multiple ornaments correlate with aspects of condition and behaviour in female northern cardinals, Cardinalis cardinalis. Animal Behaviour 67: 875–882. 163 Jawor JM, Linville SU, Beall SM & Breitwisch R (2003) Assortative mating by multiple ornaments in northern cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis). Behavioral Ecology 14: 515–520. Johnsen A, Andersson S, Örnborg J & Lifjeld JT (1998) Ultraviolet plumage ornamentation affects social mate choice and sperm competition in bluethroats (Aves: Luscinia s. svecica): a field experiment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 265: 1313–1318. Johnsen A, Delhey K, Schlicht E, Peters A & Kempenaers, B (2005) Male sexual attractiveness and parental effort in blue tits: a test of the differential allocation hypothesis. Animal Behaviour 70: 877–888. Johnson K (1988) Sexual selection in pinyon jays II: male choice and female-female competition. Animal Behaviour 36: 1048–1053. Johnstone RA, Reynolds JD & Deutsch JC (1996) Mutual mate choice and sex differences in choosiness. Evolution 50: 1382–1391. Jones J (2003) Tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor): a new model organism? The Auk 120: 591–599. Jones KA & Godin J-GJ (2010) Are fast explorers slow reactors? Linking personality type and anti-predator behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 277: 625–632. Kabelik D, Morrison JA & Goodson JL (2010) Cryptic regulation of vasotocin neuronal activity but not anatomy by sex steroids and social stimuli in opportunistic desert finches. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 75: 71–84. Kaighobadi F & Shackelford TK (2008) Female attractiveness mediates the relationship between in-pair copulation frequency and men’s mate retention behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences 45: 293–295. Kalinowski ST, Taper ML & Marshall TC (2007) Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Molecular Ecology 16: 1099–1106. Kekäläinen J, Huuskonen H, Tuomaala M & Kortet R (2010) Both male and female sexual ornaments reflect offspring performance in a fish. Evolution 64: 3149–3157. Kempenaers B, Verheyen GR, den Broeck MV, Burke T, Broeckhoven CV & Dhondt A (1992) Extra-pair paternity results from female preference for high-quality males in the blue tit. Nature 357: 494–496. Ketterson ED & Nolan V Jr. (1999) Adaptation, exaptation, and constraint: a hormonal perspective. The American Naturalist 154: S4–S25. 164 Ketterson ED, Nolan V Jr & Sandell M (2005) Testosterone in females: mediator of adaptive traits, constraint on sexual dimorphism, or both? The American Naturalist 166: S85–S98. Kodric-Brown A & Brown JH (1984) Truth in advertising: the kinds of traits favored by sexual selection. The American Naturalist 124: 309–323. Kokko H & Johnstone RA (2002) Why is mutual mate choice not the norm? Operational sex ratios, sex roles and the evolution of sexually dimorphic and monomorphic signalling. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 357: 319–330. Komdeur J (2001) Mate guarding in the Seychelles warbler is energetically costly and adjusted to paternity risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 268: 2103–2111. Kotiaho JS (2001) Costs of sexual traits: a mismatch between theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. Biological Reviews 76: 365–376. Kraaijeveld K, Kraaijeveld-Smit FJL & Komdeur J (2007) The evolution of mutual ornamentation. Animal Behaviour 74: 657–677. Krebs EA, Hunte W & Green DJ (2004) Plume variation, breeding performance and extrapair copulations in the cattle egret. Behaviour 141: 479–499. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB & Christensen RHB (2016) lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. R pack version 2.0-30. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package-lmerTest. Lahaye SEP, Eens M, Darras VM & Pinxten R (2013) Hot or not: the effects of exogenous testosterone on female attractiveness to male conspecifics in the budgerigar. PLoS ONE 8: e74005. Lakens D (2013) Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 863. Lande R (1980) Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection, and adaptation in polygenic characters. Evolution 34: 292–305. LeBas NR (2006) Female finery is not for males. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21: 170– 173. Leboucher G, Béguin N, Mauget R & Kreutzer M (1998) Effects of fadrozole on sexual displays and reproductive activity in the female canary. Physiology and Behavior 65: 233– 240. 165 Leffelaar D & Robertson RJ (1984) Do male tree swallows guard their mates? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 16: 73–79. Leffelaar D & Robertson RJ (1985) Nest usurpation and female competition for breeding opportunities by tree swallows. The Wilson Bulletin 97: 221–224. Leffelaar D & Robertson RJ (1986) Equality of feeding roles and the maintenance of monogamy in tree swallows. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 18: 199–206. Leonard ML & Horn AG (2001) Begging calls and parental feeding decisions in tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 49: 170–175. Leonard, M.L., Horn, A.G., 1998. Need and nestmates affect begging in tree swallows. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 42: 431–436. Leonard ML, Horn AG, Gozna A & Ramen S (2000) Brood size and begging intensity in nestling birds. Behavioral Ecology 11: 196–201. Limbourg T, Mateman AC & Lessells CM (2013) Opposite differential allocation by males and females of the same species. Biology Letters 9: 20120835. Linville SU, Breitwisch R & Schilling AJ (1998) Plumage brightness as an indicator of parental care in northern cardinals. Animal Behaviour 55: 119–127. Liu M, Siefferman L & Hill GE (2007) An experimental test of female choice relative to male structural coloration in eastern bluebirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 623–630. Logue DM, Mishra S, McCaffrey D, Ball D & Cade WH (2009) A behavioral syndrome linking courtship behavior toward males and females predicts reproductive success from a single mating in the hissing cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa. Behavioral Ecology 20: 781–788. Loyau A, Gomez D, Moureau B, Théry M, Hart NS, Jalme MS, Bennett ATD & Sorci G (2007) Iridescent structurally based coloration of eyespots correlates with mating success in the peacock. Behavioral Ecology 18: 1123–1131. Lyon BE & Montgomerie R (2012) Sexual selection is a form of social selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 367: 2266–2273. Mahr K, Griggio M, Granatiero M & Hoi H (2012) Female attractiveness affects paternal investment: experimental evidence for male differential allocation in blue tits. Frontiers in Zoology 9: 14. 166 Maia R, Eliason CM, Bitton P-P, Doucet SM & Shawkey MD (2013) pavo : an R package for the analysis, visualization and organization of spectral data. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 4: 906–913 Makarewich CA, Stenzler LM, Ferretti V, Winkler DW & Lovette IJ (2009) Isolation and characterization of microsatellite markers from three species of swallows in the genus Tachycineta: T. albilinea, T. bicolor and T. leucorrhoa. Molecular Ecology Resources 9: 631–635. Martinez-Padilla J, Vergara P, Perez-Rodriguez L, Mougeot F, Casas F, Ludwig SC, Haines JA, Zeineddine M & Redpath SM (2011) Condition- and parasite-dependent expression of a male-like trait in a female bird. Biology Letters 7: 364–367. Matessi G, Carmagnani C, Griggio M & Pilastro A (2009) Male rock sparrows differentially allocate nest defence but not food provisioning to offspring. Behaviour 146, 209–223. Matysioková B & Remeš V (2013) Faithful females receive more help: the extent of male parental care during incubation in relation to extra-pair paternity in songbirds. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 26: 155–162. Maynard Smith J & Harper DGC (1988) The evolution of aggression: can selection generate variability? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 319: 557–570. McCarty JP (2002) The number of visits to the nest by parents is an accurate measure of food delivered to nestlings in tree swallows. Journal of Field Ornithology 73: 9–14. Midamegbe A, Grégoire A, Perret P & Doutrelant C (2011) Female–female aggressiveness is influenced by female coloration in blue tits. Animal Behaviour 82: 245–253. Midamegbe A, Grégoire A, Staszewski V, Perret P, Lambrechts MM, Boulinier T & Doutrelant C (2013) Female blue tits with brighter yellow chests transfer more carotenoids to their eggs after an immune challenge. Oecologia 173: 387-397. Møller AP (1987) Social control of deception among status signalling house sparrows Passer domesticus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 20: 307–311. Montgomerie R (2006) Analyzing colors. In Hill GE, McGraw KJ (editors) Bird Coloration, Volume 1: Mechanisms and Measurements. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 90–147. Morales J, Gordo O, Lobato E, Ippi S, Puente JM la, Tomás G, Merino S & Moreno J (2014) Female-female competition is influenced by forehead patch expression in pied flycatcher females. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 68: 1195–1204. 167 Morales J, Velando A & Torres R (2009) Fecundity compromises attractiveness when pigments are scarce. Behavioral Ecology 20: 117–123. Moreno J (2015) The incidence of clutch replacements in the pied flycatcher ficedula hypoleuca is related to nest-box availability: evidence of female-female competition? Ardeola 62: 67–80. Moreno J, Velando A, Ruiz-de-Castañeda R, González-Braojos S & Cantarero A (2013) Oxidative damage in relation to a female plumage badge: evidence for signalling costs. acta ethologica 16: 65–75. Müller W, Boonen S, Groothuis TGG & Eens M (2010) Maternal yolk testosterone in canary eggs: toward a better understanding of mechanisms and function. Behavioral Ecology 21: 493–500. Muma KE & Weatherhead PJ (1989) Male traits expressed in females: direct or indirect sexual selection? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 25: 23–31. Muriel J, Pérez-Rodríguez L, Puerta M & Gil D (2015) Diverse dose–response effects of yolk androgens on embryo development and nestling growth in a wild passerine. Journal of Experimental Biology 218: 2241–2249. Murphy M, Armbrecth B, Vlamis E & Pierce Á (2000) Is reproduction by tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) cost-free? The Auk 117: 902–912. Murphy TG, Hernández-Muciño D, Osorio-Beristain M, Montgomerie R & Omland KE (2009a) Carotenoid-based status signaling by females in the tropical streak-backed oriole. Behavioral Ecology 20: 1000–1006. Murphy TG, Rosenthal MF, Montgomerie R & Tarvin KA (2009b). Female American goldfinches use carotenoid-based bill coloration to signal status. Behavioral Ecology 20: 1348–1355. Nishiguchi MK, Doukakis P, Egan M, Kizirian D, Phillips A, Prendini L, Rosenbaum HC, Torres E, Wyner Y, DeSalle R & Giribet G (2002) DNA isolation procedures. In DeSalle DR, Giribet DG, Wheeler DW (editors) Techniques in Molecular Systematics and Evolution, Methods and Tools in Biosciences and Medicine. Springer Basel AG, Switzerland, pp. 249–287. Nordeide JT, Kekäläinen J, Janhunen M & Kortet R (2013) Female ornaments revisited – are they correlated with offspring quality? Journal of Animal Ecology 82: 26–38. O’Brien EL (2006) Social and extra-pair mate choice by female tree swallows: the importance of male and nest-site quality, and consequences for offspring performance. MSc thesis, University of Northern British Columbia, Canada. 168 O’Brien EL & Dawson RD (2007) Context-dependent genetic benefits of extra-pair mate choice in a socially monogamous passerine. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 775– 782. Oliveira RF, Silva A & Canário AVM (2009) Why do winners keep winning? Androgen mediation of winner but not loser effects in cichlid fish. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 276: 2249–2256. Otter K & Ratcliffe L (1996) Female initiated divorce in a monogamous songbird: abandoning mates for males of higher quality. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 263: 351–355. Ouyang J, Lendvai Á, Dakin R, Domalik A, Fasanello V, Vassallo B, Haussmann M, Moore I & Bonier F (2015) Weathering the storm: parental effort and experimental manipulation of stress hormones predict brood survival. BMC Evolutionary Biology 15: 219. Owens IPF & Hartley IR (1991) “Trojan sparrows”: Evolutionary consequences of dishonest invasion for the badges-of-status model. The American Naturalist 138: 1187–1205. Patrick SC, Chapman JR, Dugdale HL, Quinn JL & Sheldon BC (2012) Promiscuity, paternity and personality in the great tit. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279: 1724–1730. Peters A, Kurvers RHJM, Roberts ML & Delhey K (2011) No evidence for general condition-dependence of structural plumage colour in blue tits: an experiment. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 24: 976–987 Petrie M & Kempenaers B (1998) Extra-pair paternity in birds: explaining variation between species and populations. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 13: 52–58. Pilastro A, Griggio M & Matessi G (2003) Male rock sparrows adjust their breeding strategy according to female ornamentation: parental or mating investment? Animal Behaviour 66: 265–271. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D & R Core Team (2016) nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3: 1-124. Potti J & Montalvo S (1991) Male arrival and female mate choice in pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca in Central Spain. Ornis Scandinavica 22: 45–54. Prum RO (2006). Anatomy, physics, and evolution of structural colors. In Hill GE, McGraw KJ (editors) Bird Coloration Volume I: Mechanisms and Measurements. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 295–353. Pryke SR (2007) Fiery red heads: female dominance among head color morphs in the Gouldian finch. Behavioral Ecology 18: 621–627. 169 Quinn JL & Cresswell W (2005) Personality, anti-predation behaviour and behavioural plasticity in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Behaviour 142: 1377–1402. Qvarnström A (1997) Experimentally increased badge size increases male competition and reduces male parental care in the collared flycatcher. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 264: 1225–1231. R Development Core Team (2015) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Ratikainen II & Kokko H (2010) Differential allocation and compensation: who deserves the silver spoon? Behavioral Ecology 21: 195–200. Réale D, Dingemanse NJ, Kazem AJN & Wright J (2010a) Evolutionary and ecological approaches to the study of personality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 365: 3937–3946. Réale D, Garant D, Humphries MM, Bergeron P, Careau V & Montiglio P-O (2010b) Personality and the emergence of the pace-of-life syndrome concept at the population level. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365: 4051–4063 Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT & Dingemanse NJ (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews 82: 291–318. Remeš V & Matysioková B (2013) More ornamented females produce higher-quality offspring in a socially monogamous bird: an experimental study in the great tit (Parus major). Frontiers in Zoology 10: 14. Rissman EF, Clendenon AL & Krohmer RW (1990) Role of androgens in the regulation of sexual behavior in the female musk shrew. Neuroendocrinology 51: 468–473. Riters LV & Alger SJ (2011) Hormonal regulation of avian courtship and mating behaviors. In Norris DQ, Lopez KH (editors) Hormones and Reproduction of Vertebrates. Academic Press, Burlington, Massachusetts, pp. 153–180. Robertson RJ, Gibbs HL & Stutchbury BJ (1986) Spitefulness, altruism, and the cost of aggression: evidence against superterritoriality in tree swallows. The Condor 88: 104–105. Rohwer S (1985) Dyed birds achieve higher social status than controls in Harris’ sparrows. Animal Behaviour 33: 1325–1331. Rohwer S (1977) Status signaling in Harris sparrows: some experiments in deception. Behaviour 61: 107–129. 170 Rohwer S & Rohwer FC (1978) Status signalling in Harris sparrows: experimental deceptions achieved. Animal Behaviour 26: 1012–1022 Rose AP (2009) Temporal and individual variation in offspring provisioning by tree swallows: a new method of automated nest attendance monitoring. PLoS ONE 4: e4111. Rosivall B, Szöllősi E, Hasselquist D & Török J (2009) Effects of extrapair paternity and sex on nestling growth and condition in the collared flycatcher, Ficedula albicollis. Animal Behaviour 77: 611–617. Rosvall KA (2008) Sexual selection on aggressiveness in females: evidence from an experimental test with tree swallows. Animal Behaviour 75: 1603–1610. Rosvall KA (2011a) By any name, female–female competition yields differential mating success. Behavioral Ecology 22: 1144–1146. Rosvall KA (2011b) Cost of female intrasexual aggression in terms of offspring quality: a cross-fostering study. Ethology 117: 332–344. Rosvall KA (2013) Life history trade-offs and behavioral sensitivity to testosterone: an experimental test when female aggression and maternal care co-occur. PLoS ONE 8: e54120. Roulin A (1999) Nonrandom pairing by male barn owls (Tyto alba) with respect to a female plumage trait. Behavioral Ecology 10: 688–695. Roulin A, Jungi TW, Pfister H & Dijkstra C (2000) Female barn owls (Tyto alba) advertise good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 267: 937– 941. Rowe KMC & Weatherhead PJ (2011). Assortative mating in relation to plumage traits shared by male and female American robins. The Condor 113: 881–889. Rubenstein DR & Hauber ME (2008) Dynamic feedback between phenotype and physiology in sexually selected traits. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23: 655–658. Ryder TB, Fleischer RC, Shriver WG & Marra PP (2012) The ecological–evolutionary interplay: density-dependent sexual selection in a migratory songbird. Ecology and Evolution 2: 976–987. Safran RJ, Adelman JS, McGraw KJ & Hau M (2008) Sexual signal exaggeration affects physiological state in male barn swallows. Current Biology 18: R461–R462. Safran RJ, Neuman CR, McGraw KJ & Lovette IJ (2005) Dynamic paternity allocation as a function of male plumage color in barn swallows. Science 309: 2210–2212. 171 Sandell MI (2007) Exogenous testosterone increases female aggression in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 255–262. Schielzeth H (2010) Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 1: 103–113. Schwabl H, Lindsay WR, Barron DG & Webster MS (2014) Endocrine correlates of mate choice and promiscuity in females of a socially monogamous avian mating system with alternative male reproductive phenotypes. Current Zoology 60: 804–815. Senar JC (2006). Color displays as intrasexual signals of aggression and dominance. In Hill GE, McGraw KJ (editors) Bird Coloration Vol II: Function and Evolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 87–136. Seutin G, White BN & Boag PT (1991) Preservation of avian blood and tissue samples for DNA analyses. Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 82–90. Sheldon BC (2000) Differential allocation: tests, mechanisms and implications. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15: 397–402. Shutler D, Clark RG, Fehr C & Diamond AW (2006) Time and recruitment costs as currencies in manipulation studies on the costs of reproduction. Ecology 87: 2938–2946. Sih A, Bell A & Johnson JC (2004a) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19: 372–378. Sih A, Bell AM, Johnson JC & Ziemba RE (2004b) Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. The Quarterly Review of Biology 79: 241–277. Silva N, Avilés JM, Danchin E & Parejo D (2008) Informative content of multiple plumagecoloured traits in female and male European Rollers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 1969–1979. Smith ER, Damassa DA & Davidson JM (1977) Hormone administration: peripheral and intracranial implants. In Myers RD (editor) Methods in Psychobiology Vol III: Advanced Laboratory Techniques in Neuropsychology and Nurobiology. Academic Press, New York, New York, pp. 259–279. Smith SM (1988) Extra-pair copulations in black-capped chickadees: the role of the female. Behaviour 107: 15–23. Sockman KW & Schwabl H (2000) Yolk androgens reduce offspring survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 267: 1451–1456. 172 Soma KK, Sullivan K & Wingfield J (1999) Combined aromatase inhibitor and antiandrogen treatment decreases territorial aggression in a wild songbird during the nonbreeding season. General and comparative endocrinology 115: 442–453. Stapleton MK, Kleven O, Lifjeld JT & Robertson RJ (2007) Female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) increase offspring heterozygosity through extrapair mating. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 1725–1733. Stapleton MK & Robertson RJ (2006) Female tree swallow home-range movements during their fertile period as revealed by radio-tracking. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 118: 502–507. Stuart-Fox DM & Goode JL (2014) Female ornamentation influences male courtship investment in a lizard. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 2:1–9. Stutchbury BJ & Robertson RJ (1985) Floating populations of female tree swallows. The Auk 3: 651–654. Stutchbury BJ & Robertson RJ (1987) Signaling subordinate and female status: two hypotheses for the adaptive significance of subadult plumage in female tree swallows. The Auk 104: 717–723. Stutchbury BJ & Robertson RJ (1988) Within-season and age-related patterns of reproductive performance in female tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Canadian Journal of Zoology 66: 827–834. Swaddle JP, Witter MS, Cuthill IC, Budden A & McCowen P (1996). Plumage condition affects flight performance in common starlings: implications for developmental homeostasis, abrasion and moult. Journal of Avian Biology 127: 03–111. Tarvin KA & Murphy TG (2012) It isn’t always sexy when both are bright and shiny: considering alternatives to sexual selection in elaborate monomorphic species. Ibis 154: 439–443. Thomas K & Shutler D (2001) Ectoparasites, nestling growth, parental feeding rates, and begging intensity of tree swallows. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79: 346–353. Tibbetts EA (2008) Resource value and the context dependence of receiver behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 275: 2201–2206. Tibbetts EA (2014) The evolution of honest communication: integrating social and physiological costs of ornamentation. Integrative and Comparative Biology 54: 578–590. Tibbetts EA & Izzo A (2010) Social punishment of dishonest signalers caused by mismatch between signal and behavior. Current Biology 20: 1637–1640. 173 Tibbetts EA & Lindsay R (2008) Visual signals of status and rival assessment in Polistes dominulus paper wasps. Biology Letters 4: 237–239. Tilgar V, Ots I & Mänd R (2004) Bone alkaline phosphatase as a sensitive indicator of skeletal development in birds: a study of the great tit nestlings. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 77: 530–535. Tobias JA, Montgomerie R & Lyon BE (2012) The evolution of female ornaments and weaponry: social selection, sexual selection and ecological competition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 367: 2274–2293. Tomaszycki ML, Banerjee SB & Adkins-Regan E (2006) The role of sex steroids in courtship, pairing and pairing behaviors in the socially monogamous zebra finch. Hormones and Behavior 50: 141–147. Torres R & Velando A (2005) Male preference for female foot colour in the socially monogamous blue-footed booby, Sula nebouxii. Animal Behaviour 69: 59–65. Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell BG (editor) Sexual selection and the descent of man, Aldine de Gruyter, New York, New York, pp. 136–179. van Oers K, Drent PJ, Dingemanse NJ & Kempenaers B (2008) Personality is associated with extrapair paternity in great tits, Parus major. Animal Behaviour 76: 555–563. Varian-Ramos CW & Webster MS (2012) Extrapair copulations reduce inbreeding for female red-backed fairy-wrens, Malurus melanocephalus. Animal Behaviour 83: 857–864. Veiga JP, Polo V (2008) Fitness consequences of increased testosterone levels in female spotless starlings. The American Naturalist 172: 42-53. Veiga JP, Viñuela J, Cordero PJ, Aparicio JM & Polo V (2004) Experimentally increased testosterone affects social rank and primary sex ratio in the spotless starling. Hormones and Behavior 46: 47–53. Venier LA, Dunn PO, Lifjeld JT & Robertson RJ (1993) Behavioural patterns of extra-pair copulation in tree swallows. Animal Behaviour 45: 412–415. Verhoeven KJF, Simonsen KL & McIntyre LM (2005). Implementing false discovery rate control: increasing your power. Oikos 108: 643–647. Vitousek MN, Jenkins BR & Safran RJ (2014a) Stress and success: Individual differences in the glucocorticoid stress response predict behavior and reproductive success under high predation risk. Hormones and Behavior 66: 812–819. Vitousek MN, Stewart RA & Safran RJ (2013) Female plumage colour influences seasonal oxidative damage and testosterone profiles in a songbird. Biology Letters 9: 20130539. 174 Vitousek MN, Tomášek O, Albrecht T, Wilkins MR & Safran RJ (2016) Signal traits and oxidative stress: a comparative study across populations with divergent signals. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 4: 56. Vitousek MN, Zonana DM & Safran RJ (2014b) An integrative view of the signaling phenotype: Dynamic links between signals, physiology, behavior and social context. Current Zoology 60: 739–754. Washburn BE, Millspaugh JJ, Morris DL, Schulz J &, Faaborg J (2007) Using a commercially available enzyme immunoassay to quantify testosterone in avian plasma. The Condor 109: 181–186. Watson NL & Simmons LW (2010a). Mate choice in the dung beetle Onthophagus sagittarius: are female horns ornaments? Behavioral Ecology 21: 424–430. Watson NL & Simmons LW (2010b) Reproductive competition promotes the evolution of female weaponry. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 277: 2035–2040. Weatherhead PJ & Robertson RJ (1979) Offspring quality and the polygyny threshold: “the sexy son hypothesis.” The American Naturalist 113: 201–208. Weiss SL (2006) Female-specific color is a signal of quality in the striped plateau lizard (Sceloporus virgatus). Behavioral Ecology 17: 726–732. West-Eberhard MJ (1983) Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. The Quarterly Review of Biology 58: 155–183. While GM, Sinn DL & Wapstra E (2009) Female aggression predicts mode of paternity acquisition in a social lizard. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276: 2021–2029. Whittingham LA & Dunn PO (2000) Offspring sex ratios in tree swallows: females in better condition produce more sons. Molecular Ecology 9: 1123–1129. Whittingham LA & Dunn PO (2010) Fitness benefits of polyandry for experienced females. Molecular Ecology 19: 2328–2335. Whittingham LA & Dunn PO (2014) Extra-pair mating and sexual selection on male traits across populations. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 126: 9–18. Whittingham LA & Dunn PO (2016) Experimental evidence that brighter males sire more extra-pair young in tree swallows. Molecular Ecology 25: 3706–3715. 175 Whittingham LA, Dunn PO & Robertson RJ (1993) Confidence of paternity and male parental care: an experimental study in tree swallows. Animal Behaviour 46: 139–147. Whittingham LA, Dunn PO & Stapleton MK (2006) Repeatability of extra-pair mating in tree swallows. Molecular Ecology 15: 841–849. Whittingham LA & Schwabl H (2002) Maternal testosterone in tree swallow eggs varies with female aggression. Animal Behaviour 63: 63–67. Williams TD, Kitaysky AS & Vézina F (2004) Individual variation in plasma estradiol-17β and androgen levels during egg formation in the European starling Sturnus vulgaris: implications for regulation of yolk steroids. General and Comparative Endocrinology 136: 346–352. Wilson LC & Swaddle JP (2013) Manipulating the perceived opportunity to cheat: an experimental test of the active roles of male and female zebra finches in mate guarding behavior. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 1077–1087. Winkler DW & Allen PE (1996) The seasonal decline in tree swallow clutch size: physiological constraint or strategic adjustment? Ecology 77: 922–932. Winkler DW, Hallinger KK, Ardia DR, Robertson RJ, Stutchbury BJ & Cohen RR (2011). Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor). In Poole A (editor) The Birds of North America Online. Ithaca: Cornell Lab of Ornithology; retrieved from the Birds of North American online: http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/011 Winkler DW, Ringelman KM, Dunn PO, Whittingham L, Hussell DJT, Clark RG, Dawson RD, Johnson LS, Rose A, Austin SH, Robinson WD, Lombardo MP, Thorpe PA, Shutler D, Robertson RJ, Stager M, Leonard M, Horn AG, Dickinson J, Ferretti V, Massoni V, Bulit F, Reboreda JC, Liljesthröm M, Quiroga M, Rakhimberdiev E & Ardia DR (2014) Latitudinal variation in clutch size–lay date regressions in Tachycineta swallows: effects of food supply or demography? Ecography 37: 670–678. Winkler DW, Wrege PH, Allen PE, Kast TL, Senesac P, Wasson MF, Llambías PE, Ferretti V, Sullivan PJ (2004) Breeding dispersal and philopatry in the tree swallow. The Condor 106: 768–776. Wolf WL, Casto JM, Nolan V Jr & Ketterson ED (2004) Female ornamentation and male mate choice in dark-eyed juncos. Animal Behaviour 67: 93–102. Zahavi A (1975) Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. Journal of theoretical Biology 53: 205–214. Zysling DA, Greives TJ, Breuner CW, Casto JM, Demas GE & Ketterson ED (2006) Behavioral and physiological responses to experimentally elevated testosterone in female dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis carolinensis). Hormones and Behavior 50: 200–207. 176